Please select what you would like included for printing: Copy the text below and then paste that into your favorite email application Enter your phone number above to have directions sent via text This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply Service map data © OpenStreetMap contributors Metrics details Actions are biased by the outcomes they can produce: Humans are more likely to show action under reward prospect Such motivational biases derive not only from biased response selection but also from biased learning: humans tend to attribute rewards to their own actions but are reluctant to attribute punishments to having held back The neural origin of these biases is unclear it remains open whether motivational biases arise primarily from the architecture of subcortical regions or also reflect cortical influences the latter being typically associated with increased behavioral flexibility and control beyond stereotyped behaviors Simultaneous EEG-fMRI allowed us to track which regions encoded biased prediction errors in which order Biased prediction errors occurred in cortical regions (dorsal anterior and posterior cingulate cortices) before subcortical regions (striatum) These results highlight that biased learning is not a mere feature of the basal ganglia but arises through prefrontal cortical contributions revealing motivational biases to be a potentially flexible also credit assignment is subject to motivational biases with enhanced credit for rewards given to actions but diminished credit for punishments given to inactions Both candidate models predict that BOLD signal in striatum should be better described by biased compared with “standard” prediction errors the model proposing a prefrontal influence on striatal processing makes a notable prediction about the timing of signals: information about the selected action and the obtained outcome should be present first in prefrontal circuits to then later affect processes in the striatum While fMRI BOLD recordings allow for unequivocal access to striatal activity the sluggish nature of the BOLD signal prevents clear inferences about temporal precedence of signals from different regions We thus combined BOLD with simultaneous EEG recordings which allowed us to precisely characterize learning signals in both space and time which predicts better learning of actions following reward and failure to unlearn inaction following a loss This finding provides a putative computational mechanism for how motivational action biases can arise through learning and aggravate with increased experience We further show that BOLD signal in a range of cortical and subcortical regions is better explained by biased than by standard prediction errors electrophysiological correlates of cortical prediction errors arise earlier than correlates of subcortical prediction errors consistent with an influence of cortical over subcortical regions in biasing the learning of actions and inactions this work provides evidence for a cortico-striatal basis of biased learning of action-outcome contingencies that may drive the formation of motivational action biases We performed regression analyses to test whether (a) responses were biased by the valence of prospective outcomes (Win/Avoid) reflecting biased responding and/or learning and (b) whether response repetition after positive vs negative outcomes was biased by whether a Go vs these results suggested that behavioral adaptation following rewards and punishments was biased by the type of action that led to this outcome (Go or NoGo) this analysis only considered behavioral adaptation on the next trial and could not pinpoint the precise algorithmic nature of this learning bias it did not provide trial-by-trial estimates of action values as required for model-based fMRI and EEG analyses to test for regions or time points that reflected biased learning We thus analyzed the impact of past outcomes on participants’ choices using computational RL models in line with the asymmetric pathways model we added both the response bias and the learning bias For the full model space (M1-M5) and model definitions which relied on only a single mechanism (i.e. biased learning from rewarded Go and punishment NoGo actions) B BOLD signals correlated positively to “standard” RL prediction errors in several regions C Left panel: Regions encoding both the standard PE term and the difference term to biased PEs (conjunction) at different cluster-forming thresholds (1 <z < 5 Clusters significant at a threshold of z > 3.1 are surrounded by black edges BOLD was significantly better explained by biased learning than by standard learning Right panel: 3D representation with all seven regions encoding biased learning (and used in fMRI-informed EEG analyses) there was higher BOLD signal in contralateral motor cortex and operculum as well as ipsilateral cerebellum when contrasting hand responses against each other (see below) These results are in line with previous results on outcome processing and response selection and thus assure the general data quality Significant encoding of both components (with the same sign) provides strong evidence for encoding of biased prediction errors PEBIAS The PEDIF term itself has no substantive neural interpretation; it is merely an implicit model comparison of a null model (PESTD) against a full model (PEBIAS) for voxels for which both PESTD and PEDIF are significant one can conclude that the BOLD signal correlates with the full biased prediction error term PEBIAS and that this correlation is significantly stronger than for the baseline prediction error term PESTD BOLD signal in these regions was better described (i.e. more variance explained) by biased learning than by standard prediction error learning When testing for parametric correlates of PE magnitude thereby effectively testing for correlations with the absolute PE magnitude (i.e. Note that PE valence was identical for standard and biased PEs only PE magnitude could distinguish both learning models both midfrontal theta power (negatively) and beta power (positively) encoded PE valence delta power encoded PE magnitude (positively) This encoding was only significant for biased PEs midfrontal EEG power also reflected biased learning we tested whether the identified EEG phenomena were correlated with trial-by-trial BOLD signal in identified regions this allowed us to test whether EEG correlates of cortical learning precede EEG correlates of subcortical learning We performed analyses with and without PEs included in the model which yielded identical results and suggested that EEG-fMRI correlations did not merely result from PE processing as a “common cause” driving signals in both modalities EEG-fMRI correlations reflected incremental variance explained in EEG power by the BOLD signal in selected regions (even beyond variance explained by the model-based PE estimates) providing the strongest test for the hypothesis that BOLD and EEG signal reflect the same neural phenomenon As the timeseries of all seven regions were included in one single regression their regression weights reflected each region’s unique contribution BOLD signal from prefrontal cortical regions correlated with midfrontal EEG power earlier after outcome onset than did striatal BOLD signal: First, dACC BOLD was significantly negatively correlated with alpha/ theta power early after outcome onset (100–575 ms, 2–17 Hz, p = 0.016, two-tailed; Fig. 5A). This cluster started in the alpha/ theta range and then spread into the theta/delta range (henceforth called “lower alpha band power”). It was not observed in the EEG-only analyses reported above. normalized to the peak of the time course of each region dACC-lower alpha band correlations emerged first followed by (negative) PCC-theta correlations and finally positive striatum-beta correlations The reverse approach using lower alpha (E) theta (F) and beta (G) power as trial-by-trial regressors in fMRI GLMs corroborated the fMRI-informed EEG analyses: Lower alpha band power correlated negatively with the dACC BOLD theta power negatively with vmPFC and PCC BOLD and beta power positively with striatal BOLD H Schematic overview of the main EEG-fMRI results: dACC encoded the previously performed response and correlated with early midfrontal lower alpha band power vmPFC/ PCC (correlated with theta power) and striatum (correlated with beta power) both encoded outcome valence but had opposite effects on subsequent behavior Note that activity in these regions temporally overlaps; boxes are ordered in temporal precedence of peak activity We thus discuss vmPFC and PCC together in the following Note that time-frequency power is estimated over temporally extended windows (400 ms in our case) which renders any interpretation of the “onset” or “offset” of such correlations more difficult these results are consistent with an “external model” of motivational biases arising from early cortical processes biasing later learning processes in the striatum While the clusters of EEG-fMRI correlation in the theta/delta and beta range matched the clusters identified in EEG-only analyses the cluster of negative correlations between dACC BOLD and early midfrontal lower alpha band power was novel and did not match our expectations Given that these correlations arose very soon after outcome onset we hypothesized that dACC BOLD and midfrontal lower alpha band power might reflect a process occurring even before outcome onset such as the maintenance (“memory trace”) of the previously performed response to which credit may later be assigned We therefore assessed whether information of the previous response was present in dACC BOLD and in the lower alpha band around the time of outcome onset C BOLD signal differences between Go and NoGo actions (activation by either left or right Go actions compared to the implicit baseline in the GLM which contains the NoGo actions; left panel) and left vs right hand responses (right panel) at the time or responses Response-locked dACC BOLD signal was significantly higher for Go than NoGo actions D BOLD signal differences between Go and NoGo actions at the time of outcomes Outcome-locked dACC BOLD signal (and BOLD signal in other parts of cortex) was significantly lower on trials with Go than on trials with NoGo actions suggesting that it did not reflect typical fatigue/ time-on task effects often observed in the alpha band both dACC BOLD signal and midfrontal lower alpha band power contained information about the previously performed response consistent with the idea that both signals reflect a “memory trace” of the response to which credit is assigned once an outcome is obtained high pgACC BOLD predicted a higher likelihood of switching likening it with the circuits formed by vmPFC and PCC (χ2(1) = 15.559 We also inspected the raw upsampled HRF shapes per region per condition confirming that differential relationships were not driven by differences in HRF shapes across regions participants’ behavior was best described by a computational model featuring faster learning from rewarded Go responses and slower learning from punished NoGo responses Neural correlates of biased PEs were present in BOLD signals in several regions These regions exhibited distinct midfrontal EEG power correlates correlates of prefrontal cortical BOLD preceded correlates of striatal BOLD: Trial-by-trial dACC BOLD correlated with lower alpha band power immediately after outcome onset followed by PCC (and vmPFC) BOLD correlated with theta power These results suggest that the architecture of the asymmetric striatal pathways might not be the only neural structure that gives rise to motivational learning biases; instead the PFC might critically contribute to these biases corroborating that PFC outcome processing occurs before the time of EEG correlates of striatal BOLD The only model consistent with our data assumes recurrent connections between PFC and striatum these results are in line with a model of PFC biasing striatal outcome processing giving rise to motivational learning biases in behavior Prefrontal influences on striatal processes might thus be a common signature of both motivational response and learning biases dACC exhibited stronger BOLD signal for Go than NoGo responses at the time of participants’ response but this pattern reversed at the time of outcomes This reversal rules out the possibility that response-locked BOLD signal simply spilled over into the time of outcomes Future research will be necessary to corroborate such a motor “memory trace” in dACC the dACC might be in a designated position to inform subsequent outcome processing in downstream regions by modulating the learning rate as a function of the previously performed response and the obtained outcome Rather than striatal circuits being sufficient for the emergence of motivational biases the more “flexible” PFC seems to play an important role in instructing downstream striatal learning processes This dissociation suggests important differences in the role of the striatum in these two processes The frequency-specific nature of these EEG-fMRI correlations further suggests that they are signatures of task-induced events that are specific to the trial phase and unlikely to reflect general anatomical connectivity while these EEG-fMRI findings on outcome processing resemble our previous EEG-fMRI findings on action selection in that prefrontal signals precede striatal signals they are dissociated in terms of the frequency specificity highlighting the distinct roles of the striatum in these processes this EEG surprise signal was only significantly correlated with the biased (but not the standard) PE term corroborating that the surprise attributed to outcomes depends on the previously performed response in line with motivational learning biases though with different consequences for future action policies our analysis controlled for BOLD signal in motor cortex an alternative candidate source for beta power suggesting that late midfrontal beta power did not merely reflect motor cortex beta Even if the striatum is not the generator of the beta oscillations over the scalp their true (cortical) generator might be tightly coupled to the striatum and thus act as a “transmitter” of striatal beta oscillations the analyses using trial-by-trial beta power to predict BOLD yielded significant clusters in dlPFC and SMG two candidate regions for such a “transmitter” our main conclusion would hold: Biased learning is present in cortical regions early after outcome onset which cannot be a consequence of striatal input but must constitute an independent origin of motivational learning biases but failed to capture crucial aspects of the biased learning under investigation Future studies comprising larger samples of participants should explore alternative implementations to reliably quantify individual differences in these learning biases The PFC is typically believed to facilitate goal-directed over instinctive processes PFC involvement into biased learning suggests that these biases are not necessarily agents’ inescapable “fate” but rather likely act as global “priors” that facilitate learning of more local relationships They allow for combining “the best of both worlds”—long-term experience with consequences of actions and inactions together with flexible learning from rewards and punishments participants saw a gem-shaped cue for 1300 ms which signaled whether they could potentially win a reward (Win cues) or avoid a punishment (Avoid cues) and whether they had to perform a Go (Go cue) or NoGo response (NoGo cue) or no (NoGo) button while the cue was presented Only one response option was correct per cue Participants had to learn both cue valence and required action from trial-and-error After a variable inter-stimulus-interval of 1400–1600 ms Potential outcomes were a reward (symbolized by coins falling into a can) or neutral outcome (can without money) for Win cues and a neutral outcome or punishment (symbolized by money falling out of a can) for Avoid cues correct responses were followed by positive outcomes (rewards/ no punishments) on only 80% of trials while incorrect responses were still followed by positive outcomes on 20% of trials Trials ended with a jittered inter-trial interval of 1250–2000 ms yielding total trial lengths of 4700–6650 ms Participants gave left and right Go responses via two button boxes positioned lateral to their body but only one button per box was required in this task When participants accidentally pressed a non-instructed button they received the message “Please press one of the correct keys” instead of an outcome these responses were recoded into the instructed button on the respective button box such trials were modeled with a separate regressor participants were instructed that each cue could be followed by either reward or punishment and that the rewards and punishments were converted into a monetary bonus upon completion of the study They performed an elaborate practice session in which they got familiarized first with each condition separately (using practice stimuli) and finally practiced all conditions together They then performed 640 trials of the main task separated into two sessions of 320 trials with separate cue sets Introducing a new set of cues allowed us to prevent ceiling effects in performance and investigate continuous learning throughout the task Each session featured eight cues that were presented 40 times participants could take a self-paced break The assignment of the gems to cue conditions was counterbalanced across participants and trial order was pseudo-random (preventing that the same cue occurred on more than two consecutive trials) Sum-to-zero coding was employed for the factors Type 3 p-values were based on likelihood ratio tests (implemented in the R package afex version 0.28.1) We used a significance criterion of α = 0.05 for all the analyses we used mixed-effects logistic regression to analyze “stay behavior” whether participants repeated an action on the next encounter of the same cue as a function of outcome valence (positive: reward or no punishment/negative: no reward or punishment) outcome salience (salient: reward or punishment/neutral: no reward or no punishment) We again included all possible random intercepts To determine whether a Pavlovian response bias or both biases jointly predicted behavior best we fitted a series of increasing complex computational models choice probabilities for all three response options (a) given the displayed cue (s) were computed from their action weights (modified Q-values) using a softmax function: the deviation between the experienced outcome and expected outcome This model contained two free parameters: the learning rate (ε) scaling the updating term and the feedback sensitivity (ρ) scaling the received outcome (i.e. higher feedback sensitivity led to choices more strongly guided by value difference akin to the role of the inverse temperature parameter frequency used in RL models): choice probabilities were fully determined by action values We initialized action values Q0 such that they reflected a “neutral” expected value for each action Win cues could lead to reward (+1) or neutral (0) outcomes and Avoid cues to neutral (0) or punishment (−1) outcomes A neutral expected value would assign equal probability to either possible outcome because participants’ feedback sensitivity parameter ρ reflected how participants weighed the outcomes they received also the initial values had to be multiplied with the feedback sensitivity to stay neutral between 0 and participants’ re-weighted positive/ negative outcome of +/−1*ρ initial action values Q0 were set to 1/2*ρ (Win cues) and −1/2*ρ (Avoid cues) which accounted for individual differences in participants’ overall propensity to make Go responses scaling how positive/ negative cue valence (Pavlovian values) increased/ decreased the weights of Go responses: Participants were instructed that a cue was either a Win cue (affording rewards or neutral outcomes) or an Avoid cue (affording neutral outcomes or punishments) cue valence (Win/ Avoid) did not have to be learned instrumentally; instead it could be inferred as soon participants experienced a non-neutral outcome \(V(s)\) was set to +0.5 for Win cues and −0.5 for Avoid cues Note that choosing different values than 0.5 would merely rescale the bias parameter π (e.g. halving π with cue valences of +1 and −1) without any changes in the model’s predictions The Pavlovian response bias affected left-hand and right-hand Go responses similarly and thus reflected generalized activation/inactivation by the cue valence increasing the learning rate for rewards after Go responses and decreasing it for punishments after NoGo responses The learning bias was specific to the response shown thus reflecting a specific enhancement in action learning/ impairment in unlearning for that particular response learning rates differed between response-outcome conditions in the following way: subtracting the learning bias κ would no longer be symmetric we first computed the difference between the baseline learning rate and the learning rates for punished NoGo responses and used this difference to compute the learning rate for rewarded Go responses: this procedure is only guaranteed to work when ε0 < 0.5 the difference term could become >0.5 and the learning rate for rewarded Go responses would become >1 we first computed the learning rate for rewarded Go responses and used the difference to the baseline learning rate ε0 to compute the learning rate for punished NoGo responses: we included both the Pavlovian response bias and the learning bias The same priors (for the same parameters) were used across different model implementations to not bias model comparison Alternative hyperpriors did not change the results For computing the participant-level parameters ρ was exponentiated to constrain it to positive values and the inverse-logit transformation was applied to ε We selected the “winning” model based on the protected exceedance probability After two localizer scans to position slices we collected functional scans with a whole-brain T2*-weighted sequence (68 axial-oblique slices interleaved multiband slice acquisition with acceleration factor 4 The first seven volumes of each run were automatically discarded This sequence was chosen because of its balance between a short TR and relatively high spatial resolution which was required to disentangle cue and outcome-related neural activity Pilots using different sequences yielded that this sequence performed best in reducing signal loss in striatum we removed the EEG cap and collected a high-resolution anatomical image using a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence (192 sagittal slices per slab flip angle 8°) which was used to aid image registration no fieldmap was collected due to time constraints an additional DTI data collection took place; results will be reported elsewhere images were high-pass filtered at 100 s and pre-whitened we computed and applied co-registration of EPI images to high-resolution images (linearly with FLIRT using boundary-based registration) and to MNI152 2 mm isotropic standard space (non-linearly with FNIRT using 12 DOF and 10 mm warp resolution) After visual inspection of the respective clusters we created seven anatomical masks based on the probabilistic Harvard-Oxford Atlas (included in FSL; thresholded at 10%): striatum and ACC (see above) motor cortex (combined precentral and postcentral gyrus) temporooccipital part) and primary visual cortex (Lingual Gyrus We then multiplied this functional mask with each of the seven anatomical masks returning seven masks focused on the respective significant clusters which were then used for signal extraction we manually excluded voxels in pgACC belonging to a distinct cluster Masks were back-transformed to each participant’s native space For bar plots in Fig. 3A we multiplied the anatomical masks of vmPFC and striatum specified above with the binarized outcome valence contrast data were modeled using two event-related GLMs we performed a model-based GLM in which we used trial-by-trial estimates of biased PEs as regressors we used another model-free GLM in which we modeled all possible action x outcome combinations via outcome-locked categorical regressors while at the same time modeling response-locked left- and right-hand response regressors This model free GLM also contained the outcome valence contrast reported as an initial manipulation check Any region displaying truly biased learning should significantly encode both the standard PE term and the difference term The standard PE and difference term were much less correlated (mean correlation of −0.020 we furthermore added four regressors spanned by crossing cue valence and performed action (Go response to Win cue The model-free GLM included a separate regressor for each of the eight conditions obtained when crossing performed action (Go/NoGo) and obtained outcome (reward/no reward/no punishment/punishment) We fitted four contrasts: (1) one contrast comparing conditions with positive (reward/no punishment) and negative (no reward/punishment) outcomes used as a quality check to identify regions that encoded outcome valence; (2) one contrast comparing Go vs NoGo responses at the time of the outcome; (3) one contrast summing of left- and right-hand responses NoGo responses at the time of the response; and (4) one contrast subtracting right- from left-handed responses As this GLM resulted in empty regressors for several participants when fitted on a block level making it impossible to use the data of the respective blocks on a higher level we instead concatenated blocks and performed a single GLM per participant We therefore registered the data from all blocks to the middle image of the first block (default reference volume in FSL) using MCFLIRT The first and last 20 s of each block did not feature any task-related events such that carry-over effects of task events in the design matrix from one block to another were not possible we added four regressors of no interest: one for the motor response (left = +1 and one for trials with invalid motor response (and no outcome respectively) We also added nine or more nuisance regressors: the six realignment parameters from motion correction and a separate spike regressor for each volume with a relative displacement of more than 2 mm (occurred in 10 participants; in those participants: M = 7.40 nuisance regressors were added separately for each block as well as an overall intercept per block We convolved task regressors with double-gamma haemodynamic response function (HRF) and high-pass filtered the design matrix at 100 s First-level contrasts were fit in native space co-registration and reslicing was applied to participants’ contrast maps which were then combined on a (participant and) group level using FSL’s mixed effects models tool FLAME with a cluster-forming threshold of z > 3.1 and cluster-level error control at α < 0.05 (i.e. We recorded EEG data with 64 channels (BrainCap-MR-3-0 64Ch-Standard; Easycap GmbH; Herrsching reference electrode at FCz) plus channels for electrocardiogram and respiration (used for MR artifact correction) at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz We placed MRI-compatible EEG amplifiers (BrainAmp MR plus; Brain Products GmbH Germany) behind the MR scanner and attached cables to the participants once they were located in final position in the scanner we fixated cables using sand-filled pillows to reduce artifacts induced through cable movement in the magnetic field the MR helium pump was switched off to reduce EEG artifacts we recorded the exact EEG electrode locations on participants’ heads relative to three fiducial points using a Polhemus FASTRAK device no such data were available due to time constraints/ technical errors in which case we used the average electrode locations of the remaining 32 participants we had a hypothesis on outcome valence encoding in the beta range We then applied linear baseline correction based on the 200 ms prior to cue onset and used ICA to detect and reject independent components related to eye-blinks and residual MR artifacts (mean number of rejected components per participant: 32.694 we manually rejected trials with residual motion (for all 36 participants: M = 117.722 four participants for which more than 211 (33%) of trials were rejected were excluded from any further analyses (rejected trials after excluding those participants: M = 81.875 we computed a Laplacian filter with the spherical spline method to remove global noise (using the exact electrode positions recorded with Polhemus FASTRAK) which we also used to interpolate previously rejected channels This filter attenuates more global signals (e.g. signal from deep sources or global noise) and noise (heart-beat and muscle artifacts) while accentuating more local effects (e.g. We decomposed the trial-by-trial EEG time series into their time-frequency representations using 33 Hanning tapers between 1 and 33 Hz in steps of 1 Hz every 25 ms from −1000 until 1300 ms relative to outcome onset We first zero-padded trials to a length of 8 s and then performed time-frequency decomposition in steps of 1 Hz by multiplying the Fourier transform of the trial with the Fourier transform of a Hanning taper of 400 ms width centered around the time point of interest This procedure results in an effective resolution of 2.5 Hz (Rayleigh frequency) which was more robust to the choice of exact frequency bins To exclude the possibility of slow drifts in power over the time course of the experiment we performed baseline correction across participants and trials by fitting a linear model for each channel/ frequency combination with trial number as predictor and the average power 250–50 ms before outcome onset as outcome and subtracting the power predicted by this model from the data This procedure is able to remove slow linear drifts in power over time from the data it is equivalent to correcting all trials by the grand mean across trials per frequency in the selected baseline time window we averaged power over trials within each condition spanned by performed action (Go/NoGo) and outcome (reward/no reward/no punishment/punishment) We finally converted the average time-frequency data per condition to decibel to ensure that data across frequencies We again performed model-free and model-based analyses we sorted trials based on the performed action (Go/NoGo) and obtained outcome (reward/no reward/no punishment/punishment) and computed the mean TF power across trials for each of the resultant eight conditions for each participant We tested whether theta power (average power 4–8 Hz) and beta power (average power 13–30 Hz) encoded outcome valence by contrasting positive (reward/no punishment) and negative (no reward/punishment) conditions (irrespective of the performed action) We also tested for differences between Go and NoGo responses in the lower alpha band (6–10 Hz) we employed two-sided cluster-based permutation tests in a window from 0 to 1000 ms relative to outcome onset results were driven by a cluster that was at the edge of 1000 ms; to more accurately report the time span during which this cluster exceeded the threshold we extended the time window to 1300 ms in this particular analysis Such tests are able to reject the null hypothesis of exchangeability of two experimental conditions but they are not suited to precisely locate clusters in time-frequency space interpretations were mostly based on the visual inspection of plots of the signal time courses by using the BOLD signal from different regions in a multiple linear regression one can control for variance shared among regions (e.g. changes in global signal; variance due to task regressors) and test which region is the best unique predictor of a certain EEG signal any correlation between EEG and BOLD signal from a certain region reflects an association above and beyond those induced by task conditions and Cz across participants in the range of 0–1000 ms We first obtained a null distribution of maximal cluster mass statistics from 10000 permutations we flipped the sign of the b-map of a random subset of participants computed a separate t-test at each time-frequency bin (bins of 25 ms 1 Hz) across participants (results in t-map) and finally computed the maximal cluster mask statistic (sum of all t-values) for any cluster (adjacent voxels above threshold) we computed the same t-map for the real data identified the cluster with the biggest cluster-mass statistic and computed the corresponding p-value as number of permutations in the null distribution that were larger than the maximal cluster mass statistic in the real data Note that these signals were selected based on the EEG-only results and not informed by the fMRI-informed EEG analyses given the topography that reached far beyond midfrontal channels and over the entire frontal scalp we used a much wider ROI (AF3/ AF4/ AF7/ AF8/ F1/ F2/ F3/ F4/ F5/F6/F7/F8/FC1/FC2/FC3/FC4/FC5/FC6/FCz/Fp1/Fp2/Fpz/Fz) We extracted those maps and retained all voxels with t > 2 These masks were applied to the trial-by-trial time-frequency data to create weighted summary measures of the average power in the identified clusters in each trial For trials for which EEG data was rejected we imputed the participant mean value of the respective action (Go/NoGo) x outcome (reward/no reward/no punishment/punishment) condition Note that this approach accentuates differences between conditions which were already captured by the task regressors in the GLM but decreases trial-by-trial variability within each condition This imputation approach is thus conservative While trial-by-trial beta and theta power were largely uncorrelated theta and alpha power were moderately correlated warranting the use of a separate channel ROI for theta and using separate GLMs for each frequency band We used mixed-effects logistic regression to analyze “stay behavior” as a function of BOLD signal and EEG power in selected regions we used the trial-by-trial HRF amplitude also used for fMRI-informed EEG analyses we used the trial-by-trial EEG power also used in the EEG-informed fMRI analyses Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will Catecholaminergic challenge uncovers distinct Pavlovian and instrumental mechanisms of motivated (in)action Modeling avoidance in mood and anxiety disorders using reinforcement learning The specificity of Pavlovian regulation is associated with recovery from depression activation and valence in instrumental and Pavlovian responding Deciding how to decide: self-control and meta-decision making Go and no-go learning in reward and punishment: interactions between affect and effect Frontal network dynamics reflect neurocomputational mechanisms for reducing maladaptive biases in motivated action Dorsal striatal dopamine D1 receptor availability predicts an instrumental bias in action learning Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement Reluctance to vaccinate: omission bias and ambiguity Attributions of responsibility and affective reactions to decision outcomes Action dominates valence in anticipatory representations in the human striatum and dopaminergic midbrain Action controls dopaminergic enhancement of reward representations Striatal BOLD and midfrontal theta power express motivation for action Dynamic dopamine modulation in the basal ganglia: a neurocomputational account of cognitive deficits in medicated and nonmedicated Parkinsonism Opponent actor learning (OpAL): modeling interactive effects of striatal dopamine on reinforcement learning and choice incentive Instructional control of reinforcement learning: a behavioral and neurocomputational investigation Instructed knowledge shapes feedback-driven aversive learning in striatum and orbitofrontal cortex Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors Neural computations underlying arbitration between model-based and model-free learning Human choice strategy varies with anatomical projections from ventromedial prefrontal cortex to medial striatum Learning the value of information in an uncertain world Simultaneous representation of a spectrum of dynamically changing value estimates during decision making Effects of dopamine on reinforcement learning in Parkinson’s disease depend on motor phenotype Frontal oscillatory dynamics predict feedback learning and action adjustment Cortical electrophysiological network dynamics of feedback learning Frontal theta links prediction errors to behavioral adaptation in reinforcement learning Cortical delta activity reflects reward prediction error and related behavioral adjustments The feedback-related negativity signals salience prediction errors Time-frequency theta and delta measures index separable components of feedback processing in a gambling task The role of high-frequency oscillatory activity in reward processing and learning A reinforcement learning mechanism responsible for the valuation of free choice Striatal activity underlies novelty-based choice in humans Orthogonalization of regressors in fMRI models The valuation system: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective value Methods for simultaneous EEG-fMRI: an introductory review Single-trial EEG–fMRI reveals the dynamics of cognitive function and tonic alertness: a simultaneous electroencephalography/functional magnetic resonance imaging study Theta and high-beta networks for feedback processing: a simultaneous EEG–fMRI study in healthy male subjects Beta oscillations and reward processing: coupling oscillatory activity and hemodynamic responses Basal ganglia activation localized in MEG using a reward task Reward feedback stimuli elicit high-beta EEG oscillations in human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex Error-related negativity predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum Prefrontal cortex as a meta-reinforcement learning system Orbitofrontal cortex neurons: Role in olfactory and visual association learning The convergence of information about rewarding and aversive stimuli in single neurons Dopamine transients are sufficient and necessary for acquisition of model-based associations An integrated model of action selection: distinct modes of cortical control of striatal decision making Increases in functional connectivity between prefrontal cortex and striatum during category learning Action selection and action value in frontal-striatal circuits Targeted stimulation of human orbitofrontal networks disrupts outcome-guided behavior Controlling human striatal cognitive function via the frontal cortex Two spatiotemporally distinct value systems shape reward-based learning in the human brain Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor Frontal cortex function as derived from hierarchical predictive coding Stable and dynamic representations of value in the prefrontal cortex Causal role of motor preparation during error-driven learning Human oscillatory activity associated to reward processing in a gambling task Frontal theta EEG activity correlates negatively with the default mode network in resting state Trial-by-trial coupling between EEG and BOLD identifies networks related to alpha and theta EEG power increases during working memory maintenance Separate neural representations of prediction error valence and surprise: evidence from an fMRI meta-analysis Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo Bursts of beta oscillation differentiate postperformance activity in the striatum and motor cortex of monkeys performing movement tasks The feedback-related negativity (FRN) revisited: New insights into the localization On the globality of motor suppression: unexpected events and their influence on behavior and cognition Polarity of uncertainty representation during exploration and exploitation in ventromedial prefrontal cortex Neural mechanisms resolving exploitation-exploration dilemmas in the medial prefrontal cortex Temporally dissociable contributions of human medial prefrontal subregions to reward-guided learning Event-related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity Event-related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: temporospatial principal components analysis and source localization of the feedback negativity: commentary Striatal microstimulation induces persistent and repetitive negative decision-making predicted by striatal beta-band oscillation Striatal beta oscillation and neuronal activity in the primate caudate nucleus differentially represent valence and arousal under approach-avoidance conflict focally modulated β-band oscillations characterize local field potential activity in the striatum of awake behaving monkeys Neuroelectric signatures of reward learning and decision-making in the human nucleus accumbens Medial prefrontal cell activity signaling prediction errors of action values Temporal filtering of reward signals in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex during a mixed-strategy game Posterior cingulate cortex mediates outcome-contingent allocation of behavior Dissociable dopamine dynamics for learning and motivation Wave-like dopamine dynamics as a mechanism for spatiotemporal credit assignment Computational noise in reward-guided learning drives behavioral variability in volatile environments Frontopolar cortex and the evidence in favor of alternative courses of action The macaque anterior cingulate cortex translates counterfactual choice value into actual behavioral change Frontal theta overrides Pavlovian learning biases Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement in Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory (eds Hierarchical Bayesian inference for concurrent model fitting and comparison for group studies Bayesian model selection for group studies Active head motion reduction in magnetic resonance imaging using tactile feedback ICA-AROMA: a robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data Is model fitting necessary for model-based fMRI A method for removing imaging artifact from continuous EEG recorded during functional MRI FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG Trial-type dependent frames of reference for value comparison Algermissen, J., Swart, J. C., Scheeringa, R., Cools, R. & den Ouden, H. E. M. Prefrontal signals precede striatal signals for biased credit assignment in motivational learning biases. johalgermissen/Algermissen2024NatComms: v1.0.0. (2024) https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10352241 Download references We thank Emma van Dijk for assistance with data collection and the weekly Donders M/EEG meeting for discussions of these results and many helpful suggestions was funded by Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) research talent grant 406-14-028 was funded by Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) VENI grant 451-12-021 was funded by Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) VICI grant 453-14-005 Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) Ammodo KNAW Award 2017 McDonnell Foundation James McDonnell Scholar Award HEMDO was funded by Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) VIDI grant 452-17-016 Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging The authors declare no competing interests reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations Download citation DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-44632-x Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: a shareable link is not currently available for this article Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science National Collegiate Equestrian Association Academic Honor Roll Primarily a 3-day Eventer from Albuquerque Algermissen has also been competitive in Jumpers In Eventing she has won the high point pony club award at Abbe Ranch and has been very competitive at the training level In show jumping she has won championships at the 3'3'' and 3'6'' level As a member of the United States Pony Club for the past ten years Algermissen has volunteered many hours to maintain an Equestrian Cross Country Course on Open Space helping her mother with breeding and veterinary work Algermissen graduated with honors and a 3.8 GPA She was also a member of the Spanish Honor Society Algermissen is the daughter of Holly Dietz and Anthony Algermissen Copyright © 2025 New Mexico State University Athletics / All Rights Reserved Thanks for visiting The use of software that blocks ads hinders our ability to serve you the content you came here to enjoy We ask that you consider turning off your ad blocker so we can deliver you the best experience possible while you are here known to her loved ones as a vibrant and resilient soul Margaret was a light in the lives of all who knew her She will be deeply missed and forever cherished Margaret graduated from High School in Texas where she not only excelled professionally but also formed lifelong friendships Margaret and Trevor left Florida and moved to Utah They embarked on a grand adventure on an epic road trip across the United States visiting every state and immersing herself in the wonders of this beautiful nation Her spirit for exploration were truly inspiring she considered herself a glamper while enjoying camping with her husband Camping trips filled with laughter and love were among her fondest memories Margaret’s journey was marked by both resilience and compassion Margaret’s warm-hearted nature and genuine care for others made her a beacon of hope Margaret treasured her role as a wife and mother Becoming a mother filled her heart with joy and excitement The love she radiated from within touched all those around her Her kind nature knew no bounds as she dedicated herself wholeheartedly to nurturing her son Mason and creating a loving home for her husband Trevor Margaret faced numerous challenges throughout her life but emerged stronger each time She loved and the relationship she had found with him and we know that she was welcomed into His loving arms From overcoming personal obstacles to carving out a better future for herself and her family Margaret’s accomplishments were a testament to her unwavering determination and indomitable spirit Her journey inspired everyone who crossed her path She enjoyed shopping and watching Movies and TV She loved her daily walks with the dogs and going to coffee with her friends She is survived by her devoted loving husband Trevor Cook Margaret also leaves behind her siblings: Ellen Carlos She was a beloved daughter-in-law to Tammy and Shad Sill and a treasured member of the Cook family she is survived by numerous brothers- and sisters-in-law Margaret Rainey Cook’s life was cut short too soon and love will continue to shine brightly in the hearts of those who had the privilege of knowing her May we find solace in carrying forward Margaret’s spirit of strength and joy as we remember and honor her remarkable life The road ahead will be long and difficult for our family as we are working to heal from the unexpected loss of Margaret and we appreciate all the prayers we have received in her honor In lieu of flowers donations may be made to our family to help with the continual financial needs of raising a baby and an education fund for Mason through Trevor’s venmo @trevor-cook-99  We would like to thank Trevor’s boss Parker with Non-Typical Excavation for their kindness and generosity during this difficult time Services will be livestreamed and available the day of services on the following link: https://www.facebook.com/RoyChristianChurch ORDER VIDEOS Terri Kamen now wishes she had listened more closely to her 10-year-old cocker spaniel Samantha It might have saved the Wellington woman a lot of grief When Kamen and her husband Michael Schulner went to San Diego in July they hired Barbara Ann Algermissen to house-sit and pet-sit while they were gone Little did they know that Algermissen would steal $26,000 worth of jewelry Algermissen came highly recommended through a professional acquaintance An in-home interview with Algermissen had gone well and raised no red flags When Algermissen came to Kamen’s home for the interview The pooch snapped at Algermissen and appeared “terrified,” Kamen said “She’s never snapped at a female before,” said Kamen Kamen and her husband hired her to house-sit again in August and September Kamen was so happy with Algermissen that she brought her gifts and paid her more than required Algermissen was repaying Kamen’s gratitude by stealing from her remained in the Palm Beach County Jail late Thursday where she’s been held since her Sept Algermissen’s first night of house-sitting was on July 18 Algermissen was already pawning their property according to the Palm Beach County Sheriff’s Office Kamen said the total value is more than $26,000 when a special event caused Kamen to wear her best jewelry “I feel like a schmuck to have been so trusting,” said Kamen The same acquaintance who had referred the 48-year-old Algermissen to Kamen had done the same for another Wellington couple The mutual acquaintance was not a party to Algermissen’s alleged thefts and immediately contacted the Harrises after finding out that Kamen and her husband had been victimized Algermissen began house-sitting for the Harrises on Sept Algermissen was pawning the couple’s jewelry Algermissen stole more than $19,000 worth of property from the Harrises Algermissen received around $3,300 from pawnbrokers for the stolen property according to the report.She is accused larceny between $20,000 and $100,000; larceny between $10,000 and $20,000; eight counts of dealing in stolen property and eight counts of fraud by false statements Algermissen is being held in lieu of $28,500 bail “My faith in humanity has really waned,” Patricia Harris said Both Kamen and Patricia Harris said they’re kicking themselves for not looking into Algermissen further A background check would have revealed that Algermissen was arrested in August 2011 for allegedly stealing jewelry from a co-worker Would Kamen have hired Algermissen knowing she had a criminal history spokesman for the Palm Beach Police Department said residents in his city are urged to run background review on house sitters or ask potential house sitters to pull a city identification card which includes a check for warrants on an individual Hess said running a background check costs around $15 if you are going to hand over the keys of your house to somebody a little legwork can go a long way to keeping your home and yourself safe,” Hess said TrendingLos AngelesAReunited: Stephen Algermissen leaves Colliers The broker shakeup continues in Los Angeles — this time Stephen Algermissen left Cushman & Wakefield in 2011 after 22 years with the brokerage and became an executive vice president at Colliers International a leading broker of urban retail properties on the West Coast will operate out of Cushman & Wakefield’s offices in West L.A He told TRD he made the move back to Cushman & Wakefield because his business is more aligned with the company Cushman & Wakefield is more of an urban firm — they prioritize it the most It comes from being a firm that has its origin in New York.” SIGN UPWhile he had no negative words to say about Colliers he conceded the brokerages were quite different describing Cushman as “a very New York firm of order and method and consistency,” tapped for many luxe dealings “The kind of business I was doing at Colliers but it was an anomaly — I felt like I was better aligned here,” he said Algermissen said Cushman had gone through some changes he didn’t like He said he thinks the recent mergers have been good for the company “I was intrigued by having more of a mandate at Colliers at the time I left,” he said “You don’t know how good things are until you leave them sometimes I maintained my friendships [at Cushman] but saw that I could do more if I worked with them While many of Algermissen’s deals have been in Los Angeles — the $60 million sale of the Santa Monica Apple store and the recent sale of the Yamashiro site in Hollywood among them — his territory ranges to San Diego Algermissen has also been involved with the sale of office properties the CNN Building (multiple times) and Kodak Campus in Hollywood the William Morris Agency headquarters in Beverly Hills and the landmark 100 Wilshire office building in Santa Monica This website is using a security service to protect itself from online attacks The action you just performed triggered the security solution There are several actions that could trigger this block including submitting a certain word or phrase You can email the site owner to let them know you were blocked Please include what you were doing when this page came up and the Cloudflare Ray ID found at the bottom of this page you don’t even know!” (Laughs) In America people talk about it People take off work because they have to go to therapy I thought this was crazy but I have to say And I think it’s actually made me a better actor this is that… Or if you use something from your personal life for work make better categories of where to put things Do you need to partition things in your life I just don’t want to bring work home anymore “Let’s go out and party!” I’m in love with my partner not with my co-star When you were growing up there was so much said about the method and DeNiro and that style of acting Oh, of course! My first movie, I was trying to hide from the camera! I think it’s human to not want to expose yourself. If you have to cry, you don’t want everybody to look at you, you know? Obviously you know you’re playing for the camera, but it’s like you instinctively hold your hands in front of your face or something. But you learn through other actors. My first movie was with Dennis Hopper and he literally was like, “You’re the greenest person I’ve ever seen.” And he taught me everything. The basics, you know? Like, don’t hide from the camera! If the camera can’t see you, it doesn’t really work does it? Do you think that will be different for the next generation of actors who have grown up with smartphones and social media? They are constantly performing for the camera in a way. Maybe. It’s hard to tell but I think so. Most people meet on Tinder, right? I was at a premiere recently and an actor that was in the movie brought his girlfriend. I was a little drunk and I was like, “Oh, how’d you two meet?” And they were like, “On Tinder!” And I was like, “What?!” I thought you only have sex on Tinder. I didn’t know you actually date. I’m so behind… Most people meet on the internet these days. Good for them! It’s so much easier, right? I feel like I was cheated to not have that simplicity in dating. Me too! I was like, “I’m never going to meet anyone!” You had to have friends who introduced you… Unfortunately a lot of people don't wait for that and just hit on people in uncreative ways. The worst kind is when I’m on location and I’m at the bar after work or whatever, and you know that a guy definitely knows who you are but he pretends he doesn’t know who you are. And he just comes over and he’s so nervous to come and talk to you, and he’s like, “So, what are you in town for?” And you’re like, “Well, you know, I’m here on work…” “Oh, for a movie – I mean, what kind of work do you do?” (Laughs) The awkwardness of it. I personally don’t like fake people. Especially in the industry that I work in, there can be a lot of superficiality. I do think you make that choice for yourself. You choose who’s around you. You generally seem to be making your own choices. Not many Hollywood actresses would use their Instagram to post a headline of someone defecating in the seaweed at a Chinese restaurant. I’ve always felt free. I don’t let anyone make decisions for me. I just opened this account a year ago, and it’s actually been really interesting to share aspects that I choose to share with the public and see the reactions. Sometimes people are not agreeing with me, sometimes they are. But I think it’s about showing or sharing an aspect of my life that a regular interview or a regular photo spread can’t do. Do you feel like you can be more bold now, also in the roles you choose as an actress? Yeah, as I get older and have more experiences in life I’m able to bring more to a role, you know? I am unafraid to ask for the things that I need to be able to perform, too. You learn that you have to speak up for yourself. If I work, it’s because I chose it and it’s a pleasure and I believe in what I’m doing. How did you get through times in your career where that wasn’t the case? Do you ever choose roles because you want to comment on world affairs? Because that’s not my role. In a personal relationship, you know, if we go out for dinner, I could tell you about that. But I think people are smart enough to judge for themselves. I hate movies that are preachy! Who needs to be told what to think? Name: Diane HeidkrügerDOB: 15 July 1976Place of birth: Algermissen, Lower Saxony, GermanyOccupation: Actress Please enable JS and disable any ad blocker A joint venture made up of four entities bought the building that holds the Beverly Hills shops of Italian menswear retailer Brioni and British luxury fashion house Alexander McQueen paying $96 million for the 11,625-square-foot property at 457-459 N The price was among the highest in the Beverly Hills market this year The property was acquired from Lang & Lang Properties by a joint venture that includes Michael Shabani of Crown Equity; Hakim Holdings; and GWP Real Estate – all based in Beverly Hills – and Chatsworth-based Mazal Enterprises Cushman & Wakefield brokers Carine Mamann Kazuko Morgan and Stephen Algermissen represented the buyer in the transaction The building is 100 percent leased to the two retailers The property features 50 feet of frontage on Rodeo Drive according to Cushman & Wakefield research There have been bigger sales on the street Bijan’s yellow store on Rodeo Drive sold to LVMH Moet Hennessy Louis Vuitton for $122 million or $19,405 per square foot Learn how to describe the purpose of the image (opens in a new tab) Leave empty if the image is purely decorative The House of Bijan’s home might soon come tumbling down The ritzy clothier’s Rodeo Drive site sold in July for $122 million and sources familiar with the Beverly Hills retail market predict that the new owner the parent of luxury lifestyle brand Louis Vuitton will eventually replace the famous yellow storefront with a bigger building between South Santa Monica Boulevard and Brighton Way would make sense for a deal that set a California retail record of $19,405 a square foot And it would allow LVMH Moet Hennessy Louis Vuitton Inc to customize a snazzy storefront for one of its many luxury brands “They don’t want someone else’s cute little building they want their own look,” said Stephen Algermissen executive director for capital markets at Cushman & Wakefield “It’s all a part of marketing and branding … That’s why it’s so incredibly valuable.”  What the acquisition means for the 40-year-old House of Bijan is uncertain The company had the property under contract for $108 million when owner Barton C represented by Holliday Fenoglio Fowler’s Bryan Ley according to two sources familiar with the deal But rather than seeking to close the transaction it went to the market and flipped its contract to LVMH declined to discuss details of the transaction the retailer’s principal and son of founder Bijan Pakzad said he could not discuss the transaction or the status of its lease under a confidentiality agreement values its spot on the world stage of luxury shopping “This year we are celebrating our 40th anniversary on Rodeo Drive and we will continue to maintain a major presence on Rodeo Drive for the indefinite future,” he said via email “There is no better location in the world than Rodeo Drive.” Just what kind of presence remains to be seen There are only two vacancies at present on Rodeo senior vice president for retail at Jones Lang LaSalle The appointment-only store could also seek a second-level space on Rodeo substantially cheaper than a ground-floor presence which has a flagship Louis Vuitton store on Rodeo as a tenant 6,287-square-foot site eclipsed the Rodeo record Chanel had purchased the neighboring store in 2013 the purchase price would equate to land value of nearly $698 million an acre The new owner can replace the existing structure with a three-story the maximum allowed under city density regulations Should LVMH choose to pursue a ground-up project on the Bijan site it would join other chic Rodeo retailers that have built flagships from scratch to showcase bold architecture and fancy amenities Saint Laurent opened a four-story shop lined with white marble and outfitted with a private dressing room on the third floor in 2014 Burberry the same year constructed a three-story building with a penthouse fitting room The fashion house is proceeding with plans to build from scratch on its Rodeo sites which together form a 15,250-square-foot parcel A 2014 Beverly Hills Planning Commission report said Chanel intended to build a three-story shop with a rooftop deck A city spokeswoman said company representatives have been meeting with city planners but have not yet submitted updated designs An attorney for Chanel declined to comment LVMH and Chanel are among a growing number of high-end retailers choosing to become their own landlords the companies can instead funnel money into a solid “If Chanel were to lose the corner of Brighton and Rodeo at the end of their lease Chanel would be out of the market for the next five to 10 years,” said Robert Cohen It’s a purchase that makes much more sense for retailers than investors executive vice president at Newmark Grubb Knight Frank “An investor that wants to put brands there and lease it is not going to get a high enough rent to pay the mortgage,” he said Opportunities to buy along the two-and-a-half block stretch are rare especially with many landowners holding on to property bought in the 1970s and ’80s a nonprofit that promotes the street as a shopping destination “People believe in in the longevity of the street,” he said By now you've heard that residential developers are flocking to Century City, but the office market has also strengthened significantly. (It's no fun having an apartment if you don't have a job to come home and complain about.) That's why we're excited to host Bisnow's Future of Century City starting at 8am tomorrow at the Century Plaza Towers Among our panelists will be CBRE SVP Stan Gerlach who tells us Century City was one of the last markets to improve Century City now has seen tremendous activity over the past few months Stan handles leasing for The Plaza (1800 and 1840 Century Park East) Recent deals include Network Disposition Services which took a nice chunk of creative space at 1800 Century Park East and e-commerce services provider Conversion Systems snapped up the Orchard Supply Hardware location in Thousand Oaks (1934 E Avenida de Los Arboles) from a private investment firm in LA for nearly $16.8M this is the highest price ever paid per SF for a single-tenant The property consists of 43k SF on five acres who repped both sides with David Maling and Stephen Algermissen says among the reasons they were able to achieve this record price was that the single-tenant OSH store is in a supply-constrained market with little to no competition The nearest hardware and home improvement center is five miles away 60k SF former Mervyn's building at Ridgecrest Town Center from China Lake 700 LLC for $5.8M or $97/SF says the property provided El Rey with an opportunity to buy into the dominant grocery-anchored center in Ridgecrest with a high yield potential Ridgecrest Town Center,a 205k SF community center at the corner of N China Lake Blvd and Drummond Ave was fully leased at the time of the building's sale bought a brand new 108k SF building in the Nasa/Saturn Business Park in Brea for $15M The buyer consolidated existing industrial locations and expanded into the new facility (408 Saturn St) First Team Real Estate's Karl Heim repped EVGA.COM NAI Capital's Tim Steuernol and Rob Zaharia closed the sale of 1425 W 225th St in Torrance Stepp Commercial's Robert Stepp closed the sale of a mixed-use property near downtown Long Beach for just over $2.5M or $161/SF The property (1003 Orange Ave) was built in the 1930s and includes 18 apartment units six ground-floor retail spaces and a 21-space parking lot the buyer has plans for a possible rooftop billboard and potential development of the parking lot NR Real Estate Brokers' Matt Pernice represented the buyer a 142k SF building in Chino Crossroads Business Park in Pomona The Saywitz Co's Barry Saywitz repped the tenant Budge's Bill Budge repped landlord Northwestern Mutual Life Insurance and Financial Service now that John Moran Auctioneers Inc has leased the 17k SF industrial warehouse for its new HQ The move means John Moran will no longer have to hold auctions off-site The facility will consolidate the firm's offices NAI Capital's Marie Taylor repped John Moran in the 10-year lease which makes musical and animated plush toys leased a 36k SF building in the City of Commerce (2839 Tanager Ave) Heger Industrial’s Stephan Ktorza and Philip Dray repped the tenant and Colliers' Steve Calhoun and Phil Norton repped the landlord Instrument & Value Services Co leased a 35k SF building on 1.3 acres in Paramount (6851-6853 Walthall Way) Heger Industrial’s Jack Whalen along with Patrick Fitzgerald and Curt Stanton with Paine Wetzel Associates repped both sides in the $1.6M deal Michael Pakravan of Kennedy Wilson's brokerage group represented Jersey Mike’s Subs on three leases throughout Greater LA They include a 1,200 SF store within a mixed-use multifamily and retail complex in DTLA (801 S Hope St) where Kennedy Wilson's Lee Shapiro and Justin Weiss repped landlord Wood Partners The sandwich shop also leased 1,200 SF at 1603 Wilshire Blvd in MacArthur Park from Wilshire Union LP and 1,700 SF in the Mountaingate Shopping Center at 1197 E Los Angeles Ave in Simi Valley from Mountaingate TT Fred Leeds Properties' Jordan Wheeler and CBRE's Scott Siegel and Lisa Engel The United Farm Workers Foundation leased 500 SF at 3002 Whittier Blvd in LA Todd Nathanson and Dimas Rangel of illi Commercial Real Estate repped both sides George Smith Partners arranged $30.7M in construction financing for three separate multifamily developments in Koreatown including a $14.5M construction loan to Elite Real Estate for the development of a 65-unit apartment community at 833 Harvard Blvd (above) including $1.2M in preferred equity from a crowdsourcing firm for Index Realty's development of a 40-unit condo project slated to break ground this month at 1101 S Harvard and $3.2M for Elite to build a 12-unit condo building at 305 S Ardmore Ave who arranged the loans with assistance from Adam Candler Hunt Mortgage Group refinanced an eight-property portfolio in the South Bay using Fannie Mae Choice Refinance facilities The early rate-lock deals include a $39M loan for the 160-unit Crystal Cove Beach Resort in Hermosa Beach; $30M for the 149-unit Copa Pacific Apartments in Torrance; and $21M each for the 100-unit Peppertree Apartments in Hermosa Beach and the 95-unit Beachbrook Village Apartments in Redondo Beach Matt Olrich and Richard Olrich from Hunt's San Rafael office sourced and processed the loans Mission Capital Advisors' Debt & Equity Finance Group secured 23 separate loans totaling $309M to refi a diverse portfolio of real estate assets owned by The Seligman Group The long-term loans are all IO for the entire term The 23 assets include 11 multifamily properties in LA totaling 817 units which received an $80M Freddie Mac loan originated by Walker & Dunlop The portfolio also includes 12 commercial properties (retail flex and office) in Orange County and San Francisco The Mission Capital team included Jordan Ray Jamie Matheny and Eugene Shevaldin in the New York and Newport Beach offices You are subscribed to the Bisnow Los Angeles Newsletter or click here to copy link to clipboard We will email you a link to reset your password Upcoming regulations in the European Union require us to show this pop-up and ask you to agree to keep using Bisnow.com We want to take 15 seconds to tell you what's going on: Legacy Luncheon: PCF President and CEO Jennifer DeVoll with event guest speaker Sherry Dewane Photo courtesy of Pasadena Community Foundation Pasadena Community Foundation Legacy Luncheon Photos courtesy of Pasadena Community Foundation Legacy Luncheon: Jennifer DeVoll and PCF Board Member George Dulgeryan Photo courtesy of Pasadena Community Foundation Legacy Luncheon: Lorraine and Lonnie Schield Legacy Luncheon: Robert Floe and Matthew DeVoll Legacy Luncheon: Kenji Funahashi and Yasi Ostovar Funahashi Legacy Luncheon: John and Margaret McAustin Legacy Luncheon: Matthew DeVoll PCF Board Member Sandra Ell Legacy Luncheon: PCF Board Member Gloria Pitzer with Ray and Charmayne Ealy Legacy Luncheon: Jim Gamb PCF Board Chair Priscilla Gamb Legacy Luncheon: Judy McDonald Judy Gain Jim Graunke Legacy Luncheon: Dave Kronberg and Peter and Anne Kennedy The Pasadena Community Foundation (PCF) recently hosted its annual Legacy Luncheon on Wednesday to recognize and honor its donors and professional advisors who have contributed significantly to its growth and sustainability over the decades The event was held in-person and saw over 100 guests in attendance who enjoyed a social gathering and a program with guest speakers and a video expressed her joy at the event being held in person this year as last year’s luncheon was held virtually due to the pandemic She noted the excitement and enthusiasm in the room as guests caught up with each other and the event was filled with many hugs and smiles PCF is a nonprofit organization that aims to build and sustain a thriving community in the greater Pasadena area and beyond the foundation awarded $2.2 million to 116 Pasadena-area nonprofit agencies and this robust grant-making was made possible by the Endowment Builders & Legacy Society Members These donors create permanent support for causes and organizations they care about in the community and their contributions are crucial to the foundation’s current and future grant-making and $100 million of that total is permanently endowed providing perpetual support for the foundation’s grant-making etc.; Hall Capital; Hahn & Hahn; and IDS Real Estate and guests enjoyed a presentation by Sherry Dewane a financial advisor with UBS Financial Services She leveraged her 25 years of experience in financial services to present “Fraud Forgery and Other Funny Business,” an illuminating talk about protecting one’s assets from nefarious actors For more information about Pasadena Community Foundation and its programs visit www.pasadenacf.org More » The ‘Humming-Byrd’ House: A Mid-Century Storybook Ranch Surrounded by Nature Pasadena Now has been published daily since April 2004 and is among the very oldest continuously operated community news websites in the U.S Pasadena Now strives to publish a full spectrum of news and information articles in service to the entire community The publication will remain free to readers and will not erect paywalls Copyright © 2025. Pasadena Now | Terms of - Service | Privacy Policy | CA Notice of Collection RSS (Really Simple Syndication) feeds offer another way to get Pasadena Now content Subscribe to our feeds to get the latest headlines summaries and links back to full articles – formatted for your favorite feed reader and updated throughout the day You are using an outdated browser. Upgrade your browser today or install Google Chrome Frame to better experience this site Diane Kruger almost didn’t win the part of German movie-star-turned- Mata-Hari Bridget von Hammersmark in Inglourious Basterds because Quentin Tarantino’s didn’t think she was German enough He was familiar with her from National Treasure but assumed she was American and would have trouble with the German dialogue and accent “It’s a testament to my dialect coaches throughout the years,” says the actress born Diane Heidkrüger thirty-four years ago in Algermissen “They eventually convinced him I was actually German,” she says but unfortunately Tarantino wanted Natasha Kinski for the role When Kinski backed out Kruger saw her chance “I can be really obnoxious when I want something badly,” she says For the audition she flew herself to Berlin and learned thirty pages of dialogue in German and in English “I knew if I got my chance he couldn’t hire anyone else.” The Berlin set of Inglourious Basterds was a long way from Algermissen “I come from the middle of nowhere,” she says “No one in my family knows anyone in the business.” Her first taste of “the business” was at the Royal Ballet in London before an injury sidelined her dancing career Returning to Germany she pursued modeling and became a top model After starring in campaigns for Chanel and Giorgio Armani it was time for a change “You can only care so much about free clothes and posing At the suggestion of The Fifth Element filmmaker Luc Besson she left the runway to try acting Working internationally—she’s fluent in three languages—she made an impression in a series of French films like Mon idole before Wolfgang Petersen cast her as Helen of Troy opposite Brad Pitt in the big budget epic Troy Admitting to being “inexperienced and completely overwhelmed” while making the movie it nonetheless put her “on the map” in Hollywood Soon she was starring opposite Nicolas Cage in National Treasure and its sequel while still finding time to make challenging films like Frankie and the Oscar nominated Joyeux Noël Inglourious Basterds is another jewel in her crown but don’t expect her to return to that kind of role again anytime soon “Every movie has to be a different challenge,” she says “I don’t want to play the same part that I’ve played in a different movie I have to be scared of it to want to do it.” Richard Crouse shares a toast with celebrity guests and entertainment pundits every week on CTV’s exciting talk show POP LIFE.Saturdays at 8:30 on CTV NewsChannel Commercial real estate firm Cushman & Wakefield announced the sale of a prime retail building along prestigious Rodeo Drive in Beverly Hills for $96 million is home to Italian menswear retailer Brioni and British luxury fashion house Alexander McQueen The sale represents one of the highest price-per-square-foot – $8,240 per square foot – for Beverly Hills this year The property was acquired from Lang & Lang Properties by a joint venture Hakim Holdings and GWP Real Estate – all of which are based in Beverly Hills – and Chatsworth-based Mazal Enterprises Rodeo Drive building were represented by Gil Dembo The building is 100 percent leased to Kering brand luxury tenants where rents along Rodeo Drive can run $875 to $1,000 per square foot per year The sale is not the highest for retail property in Beverly Hills 6,200-square-foot retail property at 456 N Rodeo Drive in Beverly Hills for $110 million – or nearly $17,750 per square foot – from Palm Beach Fla.-based private equity group Sterling Organization which had acquired the property for $55 million one day earlier Sterling Organization bought the building from The Karl B only to make a $55 million profit the next day for itself and its institutional investor partners in its Sterling Value Add Partners II fund The property is located in the Golden Triangle area between Santa Monica Boulevard and Brighton Way There have been bigger sales on this street Bijan’s yellow store on Rodeo Drive sold to LVMH for $122 million or $19,405 per square foot Rodeo Drive has the highest retail rents in Beverly Hills and Los Angeles Finance reporter Pat Maio can be reached at [email protected] or (323) 556-8329 Diane Kruger is a German actress and former model who has a net worth of $24 million She has played notable roles in some of Hollywood's biggest blockbusters and has appeared in numerous television series most of her roles have been English-speaking or French-speaking She has also been nominated for countless others Raised in a Roman Catholic household by middle-class parents Diane studied hard to learn English at a young age She attended Catholic school as a child and went on exchange programs to improve her English she wanted to become a ballerina and auditioned for the Royal Ballet School in London prompting Kruger to pursue a career as a fashion model in Paris She had been successful in the modeling world ever since 1993 when she represented Germany at the Elite Model Look event Kruger is somewhat of a rarity in the fashion world because of her relatively short stature Although she stands at just under 5 foot 6 she has enjoyed a thoroughly successful modeling career with no shortage of jobs Throughout her modeling career, Kruger worked with major brands such as Chanel, Marc Jacobs She also appeared on the covers of magazines like Marie Claire By the early 2000s, Diane had shifted her focus towards acting and began taking drama lessons. Her first film role came in 2002 when she appeared in "The Piano Player," and she followed up with roles in roles directed by her then-husband, Guillaume Canet Further film roles came with "Michel Valliant" and "Wicker Park" before one of her first real breakthroughs in the film "Troy." She played Helen of Sparta one of the key roles in the retelling of the ancient Iliad The film received mixed reviews but managed to rake in over $500 million at the box office She went on to star alongside Nicholas Cage in the film "National Treasure" before following up with "Copying Beethoven," "Joyeux Noel," and "National Treasure: Book of Secrets." In 2009 she appeared in the film "Inglourious Basterds," and she was nominated for a Screen Actors' Guild Award for her performance Nobody." She began the 2010s with a role in "Unknown," playing an illegal immigrant from Bosnia Diane starred in a French film called "Les Adieux à la Reine." In 2013 she appeared in "The Host" before transitioning to a television role in the series "The Bridge." By 2016 she had returned to film roles once again with "The Infiltrator"." In 2019 it was announced that Diane would appear in the spy movie "355." Kruger has maintained a close association with the Cannes Film Festival This began in 2007 when she acted as the hostess for the opening and closing ceremonies she became a member of the jury for the main competition at the Cannes Film Festival for the first time She would continue to appear on this jury for a number of subsequent years the most notable moment for Kruger came in 2017 when she won the Cannes Film Festival Award for Best Actress This was in recognition of her performance in the film "In the Fade," which was also the first time she had ever acted in a German-language film (despite that being her native tongue) She also won a Satellite Award for Best Actress in recognition of her role in "In the Fade." In addition "In the Fade" won Best Foreign Film at the Golden Globe Awards Diane Kruger supplements her income from acting with a number of notable brand endorsements. Since 2009, she has been the global spokesmodel for L'Oreal. She is also a brand ambassador for Jaeger-LeCoultre, a Swiss watch brand. In addition, she endorsed a fragrance line for Calvin Klein in 2010 it was reported that Diane Kruger and Norman Reedus had purchased a property in the West Village neighborhood of Manhattan for $11.75 million The off-market real estate deal was done through an LLC The home spans an impressive 3,800 square feet and features construction dating all the way back to the 1800s Federal-style architecture also makes this property stand out the couple dropped $8.5 million on a property in the Hollywood Hills neighborhood of Los Angeles and Kruger and Reedus managed to snatch it up for much lower than its original listed price of $13 million The property has gone through some major renovations by top-class design professionals although it was originally constructed in the 1920s It also boasts over 7,700 square feet of living space and sits on half an acre of land © 2025 Celebrity Net Worth / All Rights Reserved