Volume 6 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2021.709045
We examined the vocabulary growth of lexical categories in 719 children (age 13–24 months) as part of a longitudinal cohort study (the STEPS Study) and found a discrepancy in how these categories were affected depending on the child’s sex
attending day care at 24 months of age predicted a positive vocabulary growth in the lexical categories sound effects
Firstborn girls had a greater vocabulary growth in descriptive and function words
A boy attending day care at age 24 months was likely to have greater growth in sound effects and animal sounds
A family history of late onset of speech predicted less vocabulary growth in all lexical categories in boys
except for sound effects and animal sounds
Early vocabulary is of importance for later language and literacy development
Vocabulary is not an impenetrable entirety but consists of various types of words (lexical categories) developing at different tempos as they contribute to the developing language
Factors influencing early vocabulary development in boys and girls have been painstakingly studied
but fewer have examined these factors across lexical categories
let alone whether they have an equal effect in both sexes
More knowledge of what affects the variation in early vocabulary in boys and girls is needed for clinical practice and preventive purposes
Vocabulary was measured with the Finnish version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory
The effect of child and family factors on vocabulary growth in various lexical categories was analyzed separately for boys and girls using structural equational modelling
The results of the present study indicate that vocabulary development in the lexical categories is affected differently by child and parental factors in girls and boys as early as the second year of life
which gives new insights into the factors that need consideration in clinical practice and preventive work
Even in studies which have used this instrument
results indicating the effect of background variables on the development of children’s vocabulary differ from each other
negative correlations have been found between early language development and the number of hours spent in day care or the age the child entered day care
These in-depth studies have largely focused on the total vocabulary size at various child ages
but only a few of them look at separate lexical categories
This implies that precursors to limitations in sentence building and grammar can be found already in the analysis of early vocabulary content
Given that the sizes of various lexical categories are of importance for later language development
there is a need to dig deeper into factors influencing their growth
studies focusing on early growth of lexical categories in relation to parental and child factors are scarce
In the search for explanations for the variation in early vocabulary
some associations could possibly be hidden when composite scores are used for both sexes
Conceptual model of the hypothesized relations between the study constructs
The participants were part of a longitudinal birth cohort study, Steps to the Healthy Development and Well-being of Children (the STEPS study), in the Hospital District of Southwest Finland (Lagström et al., 2013)
The Finnish Ministry of Social Affairs and Health and the Ethics Committee of the Hospital District of Southwest Finland approved the STEPS Study in 2007
Participants were enrolled from an eligible cohort of 9,811 Finnish- and Swedish-speaking mothers at maternity clinics during pregnancy or on the delivery ward at birth
1,797 mothers provided written consent to participate in the study and were informed of the possibility to withdraw at any time
The recruited children were born between January 2008 and April 2010
The outcome measure was vocabulary growth between the ages of 13 and 24 months in the various lexical categories comprised in the measured expressive vocabulary
The Finnish CDI-I and CDI-T inventories consist of 19 and 20 semantic or lexical categories
which were analyzed in the following lexical categories: sound effects and animal sounds (13
and particles (10 items only in the CDI-T)
Growth between the measure points 13 and 24 months was analyzed for all lexical categories except particles
We analyzed the vocabulary in number of words at ages 13 and 24 months
the growth between 13 and 24 months was analyzed in percentage words of the total vocabulary in each lexical category
as numbers of words in the lexical categories differ between the CDI-I and CDI-T
A function factor was constructed of five items measuring different kinds of grammatical categories
All these categories express different kinds of grammatical function words that start to develop in the latter part of the second year
The families completed parental and child questionnaires about demographic factors such as educational and occupational background
and day care attendance at ages 13 and 24 months
The questionnaires were answered at gestational weeks 10–15 by the mothers
at gestational weeks 20 and 30 separately by both parents
and when the child was aged 13 and 24 months by one of the parents
Mothers recruited on the delivery ward completed the first questionnaire at that time
Descriptive overview of participants in the study
Comparison between background variables as a function of the child´s sex
t-test (conducted for continuous variables) denoted by mean (SD)
and Chi-square test (conducted for categorical variables) is presented by n (%)
The analyses for the present study were carried out using the IBM Statistics SPSS 25–26 and Mplus 8.0 software with Maximum Likelihood estimator (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2011)
Missing data on the dependent variables (0.1–0.3% per item) were handled with the Expectation Maximization procedure
In the descriptive analysis of the present study
mean and standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables and number and percent for categorial variables were used to describe the study participants
The differences in the mean size of lexical categories and in vocabulary growth of lexical categories between boys and girls were analyzed using an independent two tailed t-test
Pearson´s correlation analysis was used in comparing the relationship between growth in the lexical categories in boys and girls
p-values of less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant
To examine the between-group differences in the hypothesized regressions
While the invariance of the regressions would indicate similar effects of background variables on language development in boys and girls
the non-invariance confirms the differential associations
There was a large variation in total vocabulary size at 13 and 24 months of age in and between boys and girls
Mean vocabulary size at 13 and 24 months of age in boys was 7.8 (SD = 10.9
Range = 0–116) and 262.2 (SD = 168.1
Range = 0–297) and 340.3 (SD = 152.5
Total vocabulary growth between the ages of 13 and 24 months in boys was mean% 42.0 (SD = 27.5) and in girls mean% 54.1 (SD = 24.6)
Mean sizes of the various lexical categories varied widely between the children
Mean number of words in the category of common nouns at age 13 months ranged from 0 to 71 in boys and 0 to 179 in girls
Forty-two percent of the boys and 35% of the girls had no words in the common noun category
the variation in vocabulary size of nouns was still large
varying from 0 to 291 words in boys and 0 to 293 words in girls
1.7% (n = 7) of the boys and 0.6% (n = 2) of the girls had no noun in their vocabulary
The girls had a significantly larger vocabulary size than boys at 13 months of age in four of the lexical categories: sound effects and animal sounds
the girls outperformed the boys in all lexical categories (p < 0.001)
The percentual growth of vocabulary between the ages of 13 and 24 months was mean% 0.42 (SD = 0.27) in boys and mean% 0.54 (SD = 0.25) in girls. Vocabulary growth in lexical categories varied substantially in and between the categories. Some children had fewer words in some categories at 24 months than at 13 months of age, which explains the negative numbers in the ranges (Table 2)
Girls outperformed boys in vocabulary growth in all lexical categories except sound effects and animal sounds (p < 0.001)
the effect sizes were small to moderate (d = −0.25 to −0.47)
Descriptive statistics of mean vocabulary growth (in %) between ages 13 and 24 months for boys (n = 369) and girls (n = 350)
Independent two-tailed t-test conducted when comparing mean differences between boys and girls
The result of the Pearson correlation indicated a significant positive association (p < 0.001) between lexical growth in the lexical categories in boys (r = 0.237–0.932) and in girls (r = 0.196–0.921), except for associations between sound effects vs pronouns and questions (r = 0.182, p = 0.001 and r = 0.164, p = 0.002, respectively) (Table 3)
The correlations between sound effects and the other lexical categories were weak in both boys and girls (r = 237–447 and r = 182–365
Correlations between growth in lexical categories in boys and girls aged 13–24 months (Pearson correlation)
The first step involved testing the same factor structure with boys’ and girls’ empirical covariance matrix separately
This produced very well-fitting models in each group
Tests for measurement invariance for function words factor across boys and girls
A configural invariance model also showed an excellent fit to the data (Table 4, model M1). Further, constraining corresponding factor loadings to be equal across the sexes produced the metric invariance model which exhibited a good overall fit (M2 in Table 4)
The minor changes in GFIs indicated full metric invariance of the function words scale across the study groups
Only statistically significant associations are included in the models
Structural equation model including significant factors in predicting vocabulary growth in lexical categories in boys between ages 13 and 24 months
Note (n = 245); χ2 (60) = 122.29
SRMR = 0.016; Standardized coefficients
Structural equation model including significant factors in predicting vocabulary growth in lexical categories in girls between ages 13 and 24 months
Note (n = 239); χ2 (61) = 156.38
SRMR = 0.018; Standardized coefficients
the amount of explained variance was small
The aim of the present study was to examine if there are differences in how child factors and family factors predict vocabulary growth in various lexical categories
and whether these factors predict vocabulary growth differently in boys vs girls
We found differences in how these factors predicted vocabulary growth in the lexical categories
The main predictors of vocabulary growth between 13 and 24 months of age were a family burden of late onset of speech
and day care experience at 24 months of age
there was a difference in how these factors predicted growth in the lexical categories as a function of the child’s sex
The comparison of the results with previous studies is not straightforward
as earlier studies have analyzed the effect of factors on development of lexical categories in boys and girls together
and not separately as in the present study
Analyzing vocabulary growth as combined scores for boys and girls can possibly hide significant scores for either sexes
who found significant differences between boys and girls in vocabulary size at ages 1;2 and 1;4 (N = 903) in all categories except descriptive and function words
None of the children in the present study reached the maximum ceiling of available words in a category at 13 months of age
whereas at 24 months the ceiling of all lexical categories was reached by some of the girls
some reached the ceiling in all categories except nouns
which could also explain the predictive function of being firstborn on the growth of descriptive and function words
which could explain why attending day care only predicted the growth of sound effects in boys
Sound effects are usually onomatopoeic words which are possibly much used in play between young children and not so much in child-directed speech at home
Girls with a stronger vocabulary can presumably develop vocabulary also in day care settings without speech directed specifically at them
Among the girls there were also lexical categories that predicted no growth in day care attendance at 24 months of age
The categories affected by day care were sound effects and animal sounds
which were already more developed at 13 months of age in the girls
belonged to the four largest developing categories between 13 and 24 months of age
This could suggest that the focus and language used in day care at this early age is related more to the first developing categories of sounds
One explanation for the differences in how vocabulary development was predicted by background factors in boys and girls could be different susceptibilities in boys vs girls to the factors influencing early language development. It has been suggested in twin studies that early language development is influenced differently by biological and environmental factors (Galsworthy et al., 2000; Van Hulle, et al., 2004)
There is a possibility that when analyzing boys and girls together in relation to language development and the factors affecting it
factors that would enlighten the gender variations in early language development are overlooked
This variation as a function of the child’s sex is important and needs to be studied more
as it brings new perspectives to early language development and is thus of significance in supporting early vocabulary development and children at risk
A strength of the present study is the prospective longitudinal birth cohort design
it presents new detailed information on the early vocabulary growth of lexical categories and how vocabulary growth is predicted by background factors as a function of the child’s sex
there are some limitations that need to be considered when interpreting the results
meaning that the studied factors explained only part of the vocabulary growth in lexical categories between the ages of 13 and 24 months
vocabulary size at 13 months of age was not considered in the factor analysis; it could have added the aspect of where in the vocabulary development the child was at the beginning of the growth period
For some children with a larger vocabulary size
the growth could have continued on the same trajectory
for a child with a larger vocabulary already at 13 months
the growth may not have seemed as big as for a child with less vocabulary in the beginning
Another restriction was that the date of enrolment in day care was not asked for
This limited the interpretation of the effects concerning day care attendance
as we lacked detailed information on how long the child had attended day care
The present study provides detailed information of vocabulary growth in lexical categories between the ages of 13 and 24 months
It shows that in all the lexical categories
our results suggest differences between vocabulary growth in boys and girls in relation to child and family factors
Boys with a family risk of late onset of speech had slower growth in all lexical categories except for sound effects and animal sounds
Being a girl and the firstborn enhanced vocabulary growth in the lexical classes of descriptive and function words
Day care attendance at 24 months of age affected vocabulary growth positively in girls more than in boys
only the lexical category of sound effects was enhanced
and games and routines grew larger in girls attending day care at age 24 months compared to children staying at home
The present study emphasizes the need to be aware that not all factors influence early vocabulary development in the same way in boys and girls and suggests that studies of effects on early language development should consider examining vocabulary growth separately in boys and girls
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Finnish Ministry of Social Affairs and Health and the Ethics Committee of the Hospital District of Southwest Finland approved the STEPS Study in 2007
Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin
AN was responsible for writing the manuscript
PU was responsible for performing the SEM analysis and describing it in the manuscript
PK and PR were responsible in planning the study design and the questionnaires
PK was responsible for collecting the language data
This research was supported in part by Kommunalrådet C G Sundells stiftelse
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations
Any product that may be evaluated in this article
or claim that may be made by its manufacturer
is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher
The authors are grateful to all the families who took part in this study
the midwives for their help in recruiting them
Productivevocabulary Size Development in Children Aged 18-24 Months—Gender Differences
Google Scholar
Late Talkers and Later Language Outcomes: Predicting the Different Language Trajectories
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sex Differences in Language across Early Childhood: Family Socioeconomic Status Does Not Impact Boys and Girls Equally
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Developmental and Stylistic Variation in the Composition of Early Vocabulary
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
““The Early Language in Victoria Study” in Late Talkers,” in Language Development
Google Scholar
Communicative Skills in Relation to Gender
Childcare and Socioeconomic Status in 18-Month-Old Children
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Parental Phonological Memory Contributes to Prediction of Outcome of Late Talkers from 20 Months to 4 years
A Longitudinal Study of Precursors of Specific Language Impairment
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Early Vocabulary Development in Danish and Other Languages: A CDI-Based Comparison
Structural Equations with Latent Variables
CrossRef Full Text
Specific and General Language Performance across Early Childhood: Stability and Gender Considerations
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Consistency and Variability in Children's Word Learning Across Languages
Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research
doi:10.14325/mississippi/9781628462265.001.0001
CrossRef Full Text
How Does Early Childhood Care and Education Affect Cognitive Development
an International Review of the Effects of Early Interventions for Children from Different Social Backgrounds
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Structural Equation Modeling with MplusBasic Concepts
Early Lexical Development: Do Day Care Attendance and Maternal Education Matter
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
A Cross-Linguistic Study of Early Lexical Development
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
A Comparison of the Transition from First Words to Grammar in English and Italian
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sensitivity of Goodness of Fit Indexes to Lack of Measurement Invariance
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The Robustness of Test Statistics to Nonnormality and Specification Error in Confirmatory Factor Analysis
Impact of Early Daycare on Healthcare Resource Use Related to Upper Respiratory Tract Infections During Childhood: Prospective WHISTLER Cohort Study
Differences between Girls and Boys in Emerging Language Skills: Evidence from 10 Language Communities
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
European Commission (2009). The Provision of Childcare Services. A Comparative Review of 30 European Countries. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/social/main.jsp?catId=89&newsId=545&furtherNews=yes&langId=en& (Accessed June 22
Google Scholar
Concurrent and Predictive Validity of Parent Reports of Child Language at Ages 2 and 3 Years
Measurement Properties of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories at Ages One and Two Years
310–322.doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00146
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
MacArthur- Bates Communicative Development Inventories
SES Differences in Language Processing Skill and Vocabulary Are Evident at 18 Months
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sex Differences in Early Verbal and Non-verbal Cognitive Development
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Conversational Turns and TV during the Critical 0–4 Years of Child Development
Language Experience in the Second Year of Life and Language Outcomes in Late Childhood
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Toddlers’ Verb Lexicon Diversity and Grammatical Outcomes
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children
Examining Continuity of Early Expressive Vocabulary Development: The Generation R Study
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development via Maternal Speech
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The Relation of Birth Order and Socioeconomic Status to Children's Language Experience and Language Development
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure analysis Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
“Evaluating Model Fit,” in Structural Equation Modeling
Google Scholar
Sound Symbolism Facilitates Early Verb Learning
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Children with Language in a Speech and Hearing Clinic: Background Variables and Extent of Language Problems
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling
Identification of Biological and Environmental Risk Factors for Language Delay: the Let’s Talk STEPS Study
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kuvač-Kraljevic´
Influence of Internal and External Factors on Early Language Skills: A Cross-Linguistic Study
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lagström
Cohort Profile: Steps to the Healthy Development and Well-Being of Children (The STEPS Study)
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
A Phonological Analysis of Onomatopoeia in Early Word Production
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Father–child Activities and Paternal Attitudes in Early Child Language Development: the STEPS Study
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Size Matters: Early Vocabulary as a Predictor of Language and Literacy Competence
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Does Universally Accessible Child Care Protect Children from Late Talking
Results from a Norwegian Population-Based Prospective Study
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Socio-economic Status and Language Acquisition: Children’s Performance on the New Reynell Developmental Language Scales
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Do o Early Nouns and Verb Production Predict Later Verb and Noun Production
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hours in Non-parental Child Care Are Related to Language Development in a Longitudinal Cohort Study
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Varhaisen Kommunikaation Ja Kielen Kehityksen Arviointimenetelmä (The Finnish Version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory
Jyväskylä: Niilo Mäki Instituutti.7
Google Scholar
Continuity in Lexical and Morphological Development: a Test of the Critical Mass Hypothesis*
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Marjanovič-Umek
Early Language Development: Vocabulary Comparison of Slovenian Boys and Girls
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The Precocious Two-Year-Old: Status of the Lexicon and Links to the Grammar
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Metsämuuronen
Tutkimuksen Tekemisen Perusteet Ihmistieteissä 4 [The Basis for Conducting Research in Human Sciences 4]
Jyväskylä: Gummerus Kirjapaino Oy
Google Scholar
Ministry of Education, and Culture Early Childhood Education and Care. Available from: https://minedu.fi/en/early-childhood-education-and-care
Google Scholar
Predicting Language at 2 Years of Age: A Prospective Community Study
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Predicting Language Outcomes at 4 Years of Age: Findings From Early Language in Victoria Study
Child-directed Speech: Relation to Socioeconomic Status
Knowledge of Child Development and Child Vocabulary Skill
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Scheffner Hammer
Late Talkers: a Population-Based Study of Risk Factors and School Readiness Consequences
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Composition of Estonian Infants’ Expressive Lexicon According to the Adaptation of CDI/Words and Gestures
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Early Vocabulary and Gestures in Estonian Children
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The Norwegian Communicative Development Inventories: Reliability
Main Developmental Trends and Gender Differences
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Early Lexical Development of Finnish Children: A Longitudinal Study
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Size and Composition of the Lexicon in Prematurely Born Very-Low-Birth-Weight and Full-Term Finnish Children at Two Years of Age
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Validity of a Parent Report Measure of Vocabulary and Syntax for Preschool Children with Language Impairment
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
and Predictive Connections to reading in Finnish Children with and without Familial Risk for Dyslexia
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Parental Reports of Communicative Development at the Age of 36 Months
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sources of Individual Variation in Estonian Toddlers´ Expressive Vocabulary
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
and Gender Effects on Individual Differences in Toddler Expressive Language
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Early Lexicon and Language Skills at 42 Months
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Danish Children’s First Words: Analysing Longitudinal Data Based on Monthly CDI Parental Reports
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Girls Talk about Dolls and Boys about Cars
Analyses of Group and Individual Variation in Danish Children’s First Words
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Talking to Children Matters: Early Language Experience Strengthens Processing and Builds Vocabulary
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Trajectories of Language Delay from Age 3 to 5: Persistence
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Late Language Emergence at 24 Months: An Epidemiological Study of Prevalence
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventory
Korpilahti P and Rautakoski P (2021) Vocabulary Growth in Lexical Categories Between Ages 13 and 24 Months as a Function of the Child’s Sex
Received: 13 May 2021; Accepted: 21 July 2021;Published: 02 August 2021
Copyright © 2021 Nylund, Ursin, Korpilahti and Rautakoski. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use
distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted
provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited
in accordance with accepted academic practice
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms
*Correspondence: Annette Nylund, YW5uZXR0ZS5ueWx1bmRAYWJvLmZp
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations
Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or goodLearn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish
Day two in Finland brought back memories of a high-speed passenger ride
Words by Colin Clark
Day two of Toyota testing here in Finland and once again the sun rose early
and we were out of the door at a stupidly early time
The start was in fact so early that my brain didn’t really get the chance to engage first gear
As we fired up the happy bus to head out to the test stage I had a moment of panic
The panic lasted all of five seconds until I realised I was looking at the temperature gauge
We stopped again at the mighty fine ABC service station just out of Jyväskylä for coffee and breakfast and Heikki the talented (and very cool) cameraman decided he would join me in taking porridge for breakfast
Well nothing odd until he put two great big slabs of butter into his porridge – apparently that’s what they do in Finland
Today’s test really wasn’t very far from yesterday’s test stage but as we passed Korpilahti and turned off the main road
This was all beginning to look a little disturbingly familiar
I’d been here before and that was a very long way short of my finest moment
We came around a corner and there it was in front us
the site of one of the most humiliating and frankly disappointing experiences of my life
I sat in the co-drivers seat of a Hyundai World Rally Car and wept like only a completely broken man can weep
And who was responsible for this pitiful outpouring of emotion
I’d been persuaded to sit with him in the very same stage that Séb Ogier blasted his way through today for a bit of a fun ride
My eyes were closed by the first corner and I screamed in abject terror for all of the five excruciating kilometers of that tortuous stage
We seemed to be in the air for the majority of it but I really can’t say if we were or not because my eyes were never open
We were supposed to turn the car around and blast back in the opposite direction
I only remained in that torture chamber because
being a lovely sensitive guy and realising how distressed I was
In all his years of giving passenger rides
But thankfully the tears eventually dried up
today went a very long way to cleansing those demons from my soul
Ogier at full speed on a classic Finnish stage is a sight to be savoured and he gave us plenty to enjoy throughout today
Stage-side filming with Heikki the very cool cameraman was an absolute joy
So interviews done and action footage in the can (do they still say that in television world?)
we headed back in the happy bus to Jyvaskyla
radio cranked up to some local Finnish radio station and on comes Europe’s Final Countdown
A slightly sunburned and exhausted Heikki came to life – quite alarmingly
Belting out the words to that tune was bad enough
but as the chorus kicked in the air guitar came out
Air guitar is clearly a hidden talent of Heikki’s
The manic waving of the arms remarkably wasn’t the worst part
It was the other worldly euphoric look that took over his face that sealed it for me
Heikki the cool cameraman is a title that now lives in the past
That facade was blown big time today I’m afraid
Sign up today and be the first to know about special offers
SaveSave this storySaveWhen the sun finally rises and the snow melts away after a long
the Finns are more than ready to embrace the summer
A common tradition is to spend the season in a lakeside cottage—there are half a million summer houses in the country
An exhibition at the Alvar Aalto Museum in Jyväskylä
takes a look at the designs of such villas crafted by the institution’s namesake
an architect who made the city his home during the early 20th century
When the demand for summer houses increased
competitions were held to produce designs that could be built across the country
Architects created hundreds of drawings—some of Aalto’s are presented here in the exhibition
The show also presents models of his commissions
such as a country club built in the town of Kallvik
as well as 16-millimeter footage of Aalto’s family vacations at his own getaways
the Villa Flora and the Experimental House
Finns have sought inspiration for building from various sources
including the summer homes of artists and architects,” says curator Mari Murtoniemi in a statement
“Aalto’s summer cottage designs link the architecture of summer holidays to the development of society as a whole.”
*Through September 13 at the Alvar Aalto Museum, Alvar Aallon katu 7, Jyväskylä, Finland; alvaraalto.fi
The association said that the child had been seen at 7pm in their own home's yard
but there had been no sightings since then
Snowfall had covered the child's footprints
and a canine patrol unit arrived from Jyväskylä to help
eventually found the child in a large snow pile created when a snow plough had arrived to clear routes around the child's home
but on the second try the dog noticed something and refused to leave
Would you like a roundup of the week's top stories in your inbox every Thursday? Then sign up to receive our weekly email
By the time the child was extracted from the snow pile
In the ambulance the patient had already perked up enough to chat with first responders
The Police Dog Association said that the child had been building a snow cave when new snow delivered by the plough destroyed the cave and buried them
There was around 1-1.5 metres of snow on top of the child
Users with an Yle ID can leave comments on our news stories. You can create your Yle ID via this link. Our guidelines on commenting and moderation are explained here
Sign In
Register
Her firm faith in Jesus supported her in her illness and gave her peace
She moved to Canada with her husband and family in 1968
She was keen to start her life here and she diligently studied English while working various jobs to help care for her family
Later she graduated as a registered nurse's assistant in 1984
after which she enjoyed working at various nursing homes in the city
Raija was blessed with the gift and heart of a caregiver
which included her children and her grandchildren
She was always eager to be a part of their lives
She always lit up when her grandchildren came to a visit
Raija made everyone feel as if they were the most important person at that moment
Everything she did was fueled by her love for her husband and family
Raija was an active member in the Finnish Free Church
where she served her community wholeheartedly
She also enjoyed travelling with her husband
Raija will be sadly missed and lovingly remembered by her husband Pauli; her son John (Eva); her son Peter (Joanne)
her son Marcus (Rebecca); her son Michael (Sarah); her daughter Catherine (Kimmo); her grandchildren Joshua
Abigail and Leila; her mother Aune Kilpilahti and her sister Erja Peacey
Raija was predeceased by her brother Erkki Kilpilahti
Funeral Services for Raija will be held on Saturday
in the Chapel of the Harbourview Funeral Centre
Visitation for family and friends will take place one hour prior to the service time
Interment will follow in Riverside Cemetery
donations in Raija’s memory may be made to the Northern Cancer Fund or a charity of choice
Dialogue and debate are integral to a free society and we welcome and encourage you to share your views on the issues of the day. We ask that you be respectful of others and their points of view, refrain from personal attacks and stay on topic. To learn about our commenting policies and how our community-based moderation works, please read our Community Guidelines
County elections and municipal elections will be held simultaneously in April 2025
you can influence who will decide on important matters that affect the everyday lives of us all for the next four years
voters will elect representatives for county councils
voters will elect representatives for municipal councils.
You can vote in both elections at the same time
you can vote in different elections on different occasions or only vote in one election
See the municipal election candidates in Jyväskylä
Polling statations on election day 13 April 2025
Advance voting in Finland: 2–8 April 2025
Advance polling stations in Jyväskylä
the electoral district is the wellbeing services county
the electoral district is the municipality.The right to vote is determined in the same way in county and municipal elections
In the county elections and municipal elections
a person who reaches the age of 18 years no later than on election day has the right to vote
a) a citizen of Finland or another EU Member State
Iceland or Norway and their municipality of residence is a municipality belonging to the wellbeing services county in question on the 51st day before election day; or
b) a citizen of some other state and their municipality of residence is a municipality belonging to the wellbeing services county in question on the 51st day (21 February 2025) before election day and the person has
had a municipality of residence in Finland for an uninterrupted period of at least two years; or
c) employed by the EU or an international organisation operating in Finland and their place of residence is in a municipality belonging to the wellbeing services county in question on the 51st day before election day
provided that their personal data has been entered in the Population Information System at their request and that the person has submitted a written notification to the Digital and Population Data Services Agency
stating that they wish to exercise their right to vote in the county elections
no later than on the 52nd day (20 February 2025) before election day
The Digital and Population Data Services Agency will compile an electronic register of eligible voters (voting register) by 26 February 2025 (46th day before election day)
The information that is contained in the Population Information System on 21 February 2025 will be entered in the voting register
a polling card will be sent to you before the elections
Polling cards will be sent by post or electronically via Suomi.fi service
please see suomi.fi/messagesLink to an external website
Every person entitled to vote will receive a notification of voting rights
no later than 24 days before the election day (20.3).The card will contain information about the polling station on the election day
the advance polling stations in the Central Finland region
the addresses and opening hours of the polling stations
You must prove your identity with a photo ID
You may vote in advance on 2.-8.4.2025 at any of the general advance polling stations in Finland or abroad. Read more: Voting in advance
40100 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-20la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-20Säynätsalon lähikirjasto
40900 Säynätsaloke 2.4.2025 11-16to 3.4.2025 14-19pe 4.4.2025 11-16la-su 5.-6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 14-19Kortepohjan kirjasto
40740 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 13-19la-su 5.-6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 13-19Vaajakosken kirjasto
40800 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-19la-su 5.-6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-19Kuokkalan nuorisotila
40520 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-19la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-19Keljonkeskus/Citymarket
40500 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-20la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-20Prisma Palokka
40270 Palokkake-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-20la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-20K-Citymarket Jyväskylä Seppälä
40320 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-20la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-20Tikkakosken teollisuustalo
41160 Tikkakoskike-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-18la-su 5.-6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-18Korpilahti-talo
41800 Korpilahtike-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-18la-su 5.-6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-18Kauppakeskus Tawast
40100 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-19la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-19Kauppakeskus Seppä
40320 Jyväskyläke-pe 2.-4.4.2025 10-20la 5.4.2025 10-16su 6.4.2025 12-16ma-ti 7.-8.4.2025 10-20
you may only vote at your own polling station
at the polling station indicated on the ballot paper received by you.
More information: Voting on election day
On election day the polling stations are open between 9 a.m
Polling stations in county and municipal elections on election day 2025
Tikkakosken yhtenäiskoulu/ Luonetjärven koulu
Advance voting is also organised at institutions
Only persons being treated at or admitted to these institutions may vote there
Polling stations for voting in institutions 2025:
If your ability to move or function is limited to the degree that you are unable to go to an advance polling station or your election day polling station without unreasonable difficulty
You must register for at-home voting with the central municipal election board of your municipality of residence by phone +358 50 453 0221 on 1.4.2025 4 p.m
The county councils and municipal councils start their four-year term of office on 1 June
Postal address:City of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 193, 40101 JyväskyläContact search
Organisation
City Strategy
Volume 2 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2017.00035
Teachers and speech and language therapists (SLTs) share concern about children’s speech
and communication needs (SLCNs) but they have different foci because of their professional roles
Contemporary research has identified the challenges to schools when meeting the needs of children with SLCN
and has increased opportunities for professional development
The views of 170 Educationalists and SLT professionals in England about SLCN
and the children’s associated needs were compared for similarities and differences in an online survey that employed both categorical responses and Likert scales
Comparisons were made between teacher and SLT groups and between SLTs working in schools and clinics
There were few significant differences between the views of SLTs in clinics and education
there were often large and significant differences between teachers and SLTs
Education professionals were less familiar with terminology related to speech difficulties
did not discriminate between behaviors that might differentiate speech from language difficulties and varied in the ratings given about other associated difficulties
education professionals showed awareness of academic and behavior difficulties associated with language difficulties and highlighted associated problems with reading and writing
SLTs felt confident in their understanding of the relevant terminology but there was less clarity in the features that discriminated speech from language difficulties
Both the Educationalists and SLTs valued additional training needs with over 50% of the Education staff reporting that they had no training in SLCN
The lack of clarity about the language markers of SLCN by teachers and the requests for tools to help in the identification of speech and language problems in school-age children are important areas to address
Both SLTs and Education staff emphasized the co-occurrence of difficulties with reading comprehension and written text production
highlighting the importance of profiling children’s language learning needs rather than a reliance on diagnostic categories
none of these studies differentiated speech from language difficulties
Effective collaboration is supported by a consensus by the professionals (Education staff and SLTS) concerning whom the target population is and what their co-occurring needs might be
An initial step toward developing effective practices is to identify how the two groups of practitioners currently conceptualize language learning difficulties
their ensuing impacts on children with SLCN
it is likely that these behaviors may be an indication of a wide range of developmental problems
it is not certain which language markers can be used to accurately identify children with language difficulties and by corollary there is a dearth of information about how teachers might detect language difficulties
features of language performance alert teachers to language difficulties and the extent to which SLTs view these as features of SLCN
Whilst further comparative research is required
the current research evidence suggests that children with language difficulties have more pervasive problems associated with a range of educational and social processes than do children with speech problems alone
there is currently little evidence concerning whether teachers and SLTs distinguish between the impacts of speech versus language difficulties in educational contexts
Whether these inequalities in service provision arise because of limited resources or failing to be sensitive to the children’s likely needs is not known
In sum certain population characteristics are associated with SLCN but there are disparities in their use as flags to provide additional services or monitor progress and in the extent to which different professional groups are alert to these disproportionalities
To our knowledge, no attempt has been made to systematically examine the differences between the views of experienced Education and SLT professionals in a large sample working in mainstream settings about children with SLCN. This is of central importance to policy and practice in England, as in other countries, since the guidelines emphasize the importance of multiprofessional working (Department for Education, 2015
24) yet there remain uncertainties about the specific target population
this study sought to elicit professional understandings of SLCN to inform research and practice using an online questionnaire
The main focus of the research was a comparison of the views of SLTs and Education professionals’ understandings
the study focused on four major areas of which have been examined in the research literature: (1) understanding of terminology; (2) spoken language indicators of SLCN; (3) associated academic needs and behavioral challenges; and (4) barriers to meeting the children’s needs
We also expected SLTs to distinguish clearly between speech and language problems given their training
have a more nuanced understanding of the types of problems which occur with speech and language and clearly differentiate speech from language problems
We also predicted that SLTs working in schools (as opposed to clinics) would be more aware of the wider educational implications of experiencing an SLCN
The online survey was accessed by 350 respondents in England
Two-hundred and four self-declared as Education professionals and 146 as SLTs
A significant proportion of respondents omitted sections which were intended for both groups and therefore precluded comparative analyses
all respondents with missing data for these items were removed from the sample
leaving 170 participants in the final survey sample (SLTs n = 67; Education staff n = 103)
there were still within discipline questions where respondents did not give an answer
thus respondents were dropped from those analyses when answers were missing
The two groups estimated the percentage of pupils that they worked with who had: special education needs (SEN); learned EAL; were eligible for the pupil premium; or who required support for SLCN
“Don’t know” responses (<5%) were removed and the remaining responses collapsed into “<25%,” “between 26 and 75%,” and “more than 75%” (Table S5 in Supplementary Material)
There were significant associations between respondent group and: pupils with: SEN
Φc = 0.67 working across a range of key stages (Table S4 in Supplementary Material)
The SLTs reported working with more pupils with SEN and SLCN than the Education group whereas the Education respondents reported working with more pupils with EAL
There was no association for pupil premium [χ2 (2
The SLTs were initially divided into three subgroups based on where they worked: education; clinic; student
When aspects of the study focused on SLTs in practice
There were 48 SLTs in education and 17 SLTs in clinics
and work with pupil groups (see Tables S1–S3 in Supplementary Material)
There were no significant associations between the SLT subgroups and number of pupils with SEN
and SLCN that they worked with (Tables S6 and S7 in Supplementary Material)
Subsequent analyses compared responses from SLTs working in education and clinics
as there were no statistically significant differences on any measure the results are not presented here but are available from the authors
Only respondents in Education settings were asked about their patterns of working with SLTs
Almost all (88.35%) of the respondents in the Education group indicated that they had access to SLTs in their school where the modal frequency of visits by SLTs to schools was weekly (36.9%
see Table S8 in Supplementary Material for details)
Those with access to SLTs also reported that about a quarter (25.2%) of the SLTs worked in classrooms
and training providers working for children with SLCN were contacted and asked to distribute the link to the survey
Participation was voluntary and responses were anonymized
The deadline for completion was indicated on the survey
Two reminders were sent before the survey was closed
Email lists for both groups of professionals were used to identify and contact potential participants and the people contacted were also asked to pass the link on to other appropriate parties
As participation was voluntary and the details of the distribution lists were confidential
there was no control over who completed the survey
all participants provided background information about their professional status
contact time with children (only for SLTs)
This provided indicative data of the representativeness of the sample
Ethical agreement was provided for the study by the UCL
Institute of Education Research Ethics Committee
which adheres to the British Psychological Society guidelines
The survey was based on a review of the research literature that related to SLCN, group interviews with Special Educational Needs coordinators (SENCos) and a pilot study with SENCos who were not respondents in the present study (see Dockrell and Howell, 2015)
Items which were ambiguous or failed to discriminate across respondents in the pilot work were eliminated
Given that the aim was to compare teachers and SLTs
a preliminary version of the survey was given separately to a group of SLTs and experienced teachers
Amendments were made to clarify/edit questions and to modify response options
as indications of consistency of use of all scales
The majority of items required respondents to indicate their views on Likert scales
and communication needs (SLCN) terminology (M and SD) for the education and speech and language therapist groups.a
Percentage of respondents in the education and speech and language therapist (SLT) groups who reported difficulties which were either a feature of a speech difficulty
Reported associated difficulties [M (SD)] for children with speech difficulties for education and speech and language therapist (SLT) groups.a
Reported associated problems [M (SD)] for children with language difficulties for education and speech and language therapist (SLT) groups
The dependent variables were binary when categorical decisions about demographic information and the relationship between specific language behaviors and speech/language difficulties were collected
The remaining items required respondents to indicate their views on three-
Our aim was to compare SLTs and Education staff with respect to understandings and working practice for children with SLCN
the primary data analyses compared responses between the two groups (χ2 for categorical data
MANOVA for scaled responses) with group as the independent variable in all cases
patterns within groups were analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVAs
all ANOVAs are reported after Greenhouse Geisser corrections
post hoc tests are computed using Bonferroni corrections
Effect sizes quantify the size of the difference between the two groups and by convention effect sizes above 0.80 are large
To examine whether the two groups could be differentiated by their responses about the language behaviors, a recursive partitioning and regression tree analysis were conducted using the rpart package (Therneau et al., 2015) in R (R Core Team, 2017)
This works by first finding the variable which best splits the data into the two groups
as achieved by minimizing generalized Gini impurity
The ideal value for this measure is 0 when all cases in a given group belong to one category
The process is then repeated recursively on the resulting groups using remaining variables until no further splits can be made
The results are presented in five sections reflecting the research questions
Respondents rated their familiarity with 14 terms used to refer to SLCNs (see Table 1 and Table S9 in Supplementary Material)
Cronbach’s α for the 14 terms was 0.96
the responses given by the SLT group were virtually at ceiling consequently
analysis to compare respondent groups statistically to determine differences in familiarity with terms could not be performed for them
familiarity differed significantly across the terms for the Education respondents [F(8.26
η2 = 0.22]; respondents were most familiar with SLCN and least familiar with oro-motor dyspraxia (p < 0.05)
all between group (Education and SLT) comparisons were significant
The classification tree in Figure 1 depicts the pattern of answers about language behaviors that best differentiated between respondents from the two groups. The response options are designated S for speech difficulty, L for language difficulty, B for both speech and language difficulties, N for neither type of difficulty, and D for unsure (do not know). Each branch in Figure 1 lists the behavior and
and D) given to that behavior which bifurcates the groups
The response options that are omitted for each behavior do not discriminate between the two groups
Figure 1. Classification tree indicating how response options about the first seven language behaviors in Table 2 differentiated between the education and speech and language therapist groups
The first behavior identified was circumlocution. This is consistent with the χ2 analysis shown in Table 2 in which circumlocution showed the biggest difference between groups (as indicated by the largest χ2 value and smallest p)
Respondents who tended to use “not sure,” “neither speech nor language difficulties,” or “speech difficulties” for this behavior were likely to be from the Education group (oval box at left)
the Education group was unsure what type of difficulty circumlocution was as reflected in the response options that they employed
Looking across behaviors used in the classification tree
the “not sure” (D) response option was frequently given by the Education
This provides further evidence that Education staff are not confident in their knowledge of speech and language difficulties
SLTs did not make a clear differentiation between problems reflecting language and those reflecting speech
perhaps reflecting the co-occurrence of speech and language difficulties but further raising questions about criteria used for differential diagnosis
Cronbach’s α across the 13 features was 0.79
Mean ratings in the following ranges were designated as “likely” (1–2.2)
“moderately likely” (2.3–3.5)
or “unlikely” (above 3.6) occurrences
The majority of behaviors were seen by both groups as moderately likely co-occurrences with speech difficulties
We first examined the effect of group membership on respondents’ views of educational and behavioral and social difficulties in children with speech problems using a MANOVA. Means (SDs) and statistical results are presented in Table 3
There was a significant difference with a small effect size for educational difficulties
There was a similar significant group effect with a small effect size for behavioral difficulties
the Education group reported that difficulties with anxiety
and self-esteem were more likely co-occurrences for children with speech problems than did the SLTs
within-group differences on their ratings were examined with a repeated-measures ANOVA
There was a within-group difference for educational difficulties in the Education respondents with a medium effect size [F(2.519
η2 = 0.42]; “reading decoding” and “reading comprehension” differed significantly from “spelling” (p = 0.006 and p < 0.001
where spelling was the most commonly reported co-occurrence with speech difficulties for the Education respondents
“Reading comprehension” was a less likely reported co-occurrence than “writing texts” (p < 0.001)
For the behavioral difficulties in children with speech difficulties
there was also a significant within-group difference for the Education respondents with a medium effect size [F(5.179
η2 = 0.52]; responses indicating “bullying other children” was a more unlikely co-occurrence of speech difficulties than all of the other behavioral difficulties (p < 0.01)
Similar repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted with the SLT group for academic and behavioral difficulties
large and significant differences were found [academic difficulties F(2.030
η2 = 0.86; behavioral difficulties F(4.414
“reading comprehension” and “writing texts” were viewed as less likely co-occurrences than “reading decoding” and “spelling.” For behavioral difficulties
similar to the findings with Education staff
responses indicating “bullying other children” was a more unlikely co-occurrence of speech difficulties than all of the other behavioral difficulties (p < 0.001) apart from “depression” (p = 0.975)
Although both SLTs and Education staff were aware of the impact of speech problems on learning and behavior
Teachers were more alert to the wider ramifications of speech problems both in terms of the curriculum and in terms of confidence and self-esteem
Overall both groups of respondents felt that difficulties were moderately likely to co-occur with speech problems but within-groups analyses demonstrated that the patterns of problems varied
There was a significant effect of group for educational difficulties for children with language difficulties
Responses differed for both “reading decoding” and “spelling” Education staff considered that these were more likely co-occurrences for children with language difficulties than did SLTs
There was no significant group effect for behavior problems
There was also a within-group difference for the Education respondents for educational problems for children with language difficulties with a moderate effect size [F(2.587
Both “reading comprehension” and “writing texts” were viewed as more likely co-occurrences with language difficulties than “reading decoding” and “spelling” (p < 0.001)
There was also a large and significant within-group effect for behavioral difficulties [F(4.704
η2 = 0.80] where Education staff reported that “bullying other children” was the least likely co-occurrence with language difficulties (for all other behavior difficulties p < 0.001)
Within-group analyses for the SLTs showed a large and significant difference for academic difficulties
η2 = 0.80 and behavioral difficulties
A similar pattern to the Education respondents was evident where both “reading comprehension” and “writing texts” were rated as more common occurrences than
“reading decoding” and “spelling” for children with language difficulties (p < 0.001)
“bullying other children” was significantly different from responses to all other behaviors (p < 0.01) except for “depression” (p = 0.07)
Both SLTs and Education staff were aware of the impact of language problems on learning and behavior
respondents felt that difficulties were highly likely to co-occur with language problems
the two groups did not differ in their views of the likelihood of different patterns of behavior problems but there were significant differences in their views of academic difficulties where Education staff were more alert to reading decoding and spelling problems
Respondents assessed whether language and speech difficulties were more frequent for children from areas of social disadvantage
“Don’t know” responses were excluded before analyses were conducted
The majority of respondents from the Education and SLT groups reported that language difficulties were more common in children from areas of social disadvantage (90.4 and 98.5%
respectively) and for children with SEN (93.8%; 98.5%)
neither group considered that language difficulties were encountered more frequently when children spoke EAL (36.3%; 21.3%) (see Table S10 in Supplementary Material)
There were associations between respondent group and responses for children with social disadvantage [χ2(1
Φc = 0.16] and EAL [χ2(1
SLTs were more likely to associate language difficulties with social disadvantage and Education respondents with EAL
There was no difference between groups for SEN [χ2(1
There were significant differences in the reported likelihood of speech difficulties for children with EAL [Education
p < 0.001,Φc = 0.30]; but not for social disadvantage [Education
Φc = 0.08]; or SEN [Education
Details can be found in Tables S10 and S11 in Supplementary Material
To identify barriers to meeting the children’s needs, we considered previous training and professional development, respondents’ confidence, and resource needs. Mean responses and percentage responses to barriers are reported in Table 5
Responses that indicated that the topic was “not applicable” were removed before analysis (<5%)
the biggest barrier was lack of training in relevant areas
Barriers to practice [%(n)] for Education (n = 94) and speech and language therapists (SLTs) (n = 62)
there was a significant difference in ratings across response in a repeated-measures ANOVA [F(4.856
Responses to “lack of training in relevant areas” had the highest mean and all the comparisons between this option and the rest were significant (p < 0.01) apart from the comparison with “difficulties in knowing how to support children with SLCN.” SLT responses also differed significantly across options [F(4.864
Responses to the “lack of training in relevant areas” option had the highest mean and were significant in all comparisons (p < 0.01) except for “difficulties in identifying children with SLCN.”
Six questions related to initial training and specific guidance for children with SLCN were addressed to the Education group only (Table 6)
More than half of the respondents reported that they had not received any of the forms of guidance for SLCN during their professional training
Percentage of respondents in the Education group reporting information and guidance received during their professional training
The Education group was asked how confident they were about dealing with children with speech and language difficulties using a five-point scale (1 was entirely confident and 5 was not at all confident)
The six items were: children with language difficulties; children with speech difficulties; professional support for children with language difficulties; professional support for children with speech difficulties; resources to support children with language difficulties; and resources to support children with speech difficulties
Mean responses to all six events were below 3 showing some confidence about access to resources in relation to children with SLCN but confidence varied significantly across options [F(3.571
Confidence about identifying resources to support children with speech difficulties was significantly lower than confidence about professional support for children with language difficulties (p = 0.001)
professional support for children with speech difficulties (p = 0.006) and resources to support children with language difficulties (p = 0.004)
Respondents were asked how helpful specific resources were for supporting children with SLCN
They were most positive about receiving specific guidance (82%) and least positive about receiving information about a diagnosis (52%)
All respondents were asked whether a tool for identifying children with language difficulties would be useful
There were significant differences between response and respondent group
Details are given in Table S12 in Supplementary Material
A similar analysis to establish the usefulness of a tool for identifying children with speech difficulties also showed a significant association between the factors of group and useful/not useful
the Education group considered that such tools would be more useful than did the SLT group
Experienced practitioners responded to the survey
moreover the majority of the respondents from the Education group had experience of SLTs working in their schools
We established that the groups differed in the language behaviors that they reported indicated a speech or language difficulty
with Education practitioners expressing less certainty of what language behaviors are indicative of an SLCN
language difficulties were reported to have more serious repercussions for academic problems and social/emotional problems than speech problems
the patterns of severity of impact varied for both the Education and SLT groups
Education practitioners reported a greater awareness of the emotional difficulties experienced by children with speech problems
Both groups highlighted lack of training as a significant barrier to meeting the children’s needs
a finding consistent with the Education respondents’ reports of their lack of training about SLCN
The results indicated that identifying resources to help children with speech difficulties was a particular area of concern for the Education group
but not the SLTS emphasized the need for tools to support them in the identification of children with speech and language difficulties
To address the limited information available about the differences and similarities in the understanding of SLTs and Educational practitioners about SLCN
we administered a bespoke questionnaire which captured understandings of terminology
and professional barriers to meeting the children’s needs
We had predicted that SLTs working in education
would be more aware of the educational difficulties experienced by children with SLCN
We found no significant differences between the two groups of SLTS on any measure
as predicted Education staff were less familiar than SLTs with current terminology surrounding different speech and language problems and were less aware of different proposed language markers of SLCN
The lack of familiarity with terminology related to speech difficulties by Education respondents is an unexpected and
It was also anticipated that Education staff would have a better awareness of the range of educational and behavioral problems that are often experienced by the children with SLCN
The results did not conform this prediction and demonstrated a more nuanced situation that varied between speech and language and academic and behavioral challenges
Despite raised concerns about language difficulties no more than one-third of the Education respondents had received guidance or training in relation to SLCN and the both groups differed in the ways to address this gap
Education respondents rated the availability of tools to identify speech and language difficulties highly
each of the areas investigated is examined drawing implications for research and service development
They do highlight the importance of developing models of service provision that address disproportionalities
these problems included problems with both confidence and self-esteem
Given the importance of social emotional and mental health factors for supporting the adjustment of children the teachers’ insights point to an important gap in current support services for children with speech difficulties
Again these data support the importance of developing service delivery models that are premised on collaboration so as to deliver effective practice to meet all the needs of the child with an SLCN
van den Bedem and colleagues (under review) have argued that this reflects difficulties with understanding emotions rather than understanding language
or other) due to deficits in comprehension or production” and language abilities that are “substantially and quantifiably” below age expectations
Allowing for a broader reflection of language disorders would allow both teachers and therapists a wider frame of reference and
allow for more accurate identification of interventions
the views of the staff from education point to a wider profiling of children’s language learning needs
Why SLTs were reticent about the approach is not clear, though it may reflect the current lack of evidence about screening for speech or language delay (Wallace et al., 2015)
The development of a tool which goes beyond the early years to support school staff could be beneficial in providing them with key markers of effective speaking and listening in school-age children and for highlighting pupils who may need further support either in the classroom or by referral to external professionals
The development of effective services for children with SLCN depends on both mapping provision and examining professionals’ understanding so as to develop effective collaborative practice
we compared the views of experienced SLTs and Education practitioners
The survey demonstrated differences in the understandings of speech and language difficulties but also importantly in ways of removing barriers to effective practice
While there is clearly scope for further training the results also highlight the ways in which the working context influences practice and understandings and the need for bespoke resources to meet the needs in those contexts to meet the children’s needs
Participation was anonymous and voluntary through an online survey
JD and PH conceptualized and conducted the study
AF advised on a completed the classification tree analysis
This project was funded by a UCL/IOE collaboration award
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/feduc.2017.00035/full#supplementary-material
Teacher identification of children at risk for language impairment in the first year of school
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
Google Scholar
The role of social cognition and prosocial behaviour in relation to the socio-emotional functioning of primary aged children with specific language impairment
Speech and language therapists and teachers working together: exploring the issues
Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems
CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary delphi consensus study
Identifying language impairments in children
Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: terminology
Fluency of school-aged children with a history of specific expressive language impairment: an exploratory study
Profiles of emergent literacy skills among preschool children who are at risk for academic difficulties
Communication Trust. (2017). Professional Development in Speech Language and Communication. Available at: http://www.thecommunicationtrust.org.uk/media/526748/1._tct_workforce_development_report_final_online.pdf
Google Scholar
Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI)
Specific language impairment and school outcomes
I: identifying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory education
Number skills and knowledge in children with specific language impairment
Sociodynamic relationships between children who stutter and their non-stuttering classmates
Google Scholar
Department of Education and Skills
Special Educational Needs Code of Practice
Google Scholar
Failure to meet language milestones at two years of age is predictive of specific language impairment
language and communication needs: an overview of the results of the Better Communication Research Programme
Identifying the challenges and opportunities to meet the needs of children with speech
Children with specific speech and language difficulties – the teachers’ perspective
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Measurement issues: assessing language skills in young children
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The impact of specific language impairment on adolescents’ written text
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Explaining the academic achievement at school leaving for pupils with a history of language impairment: previous academic achievement and literacy skills
The role of language in mathematical development: evidence from children with specific language impairments
Early Years Foundation Stage profile (EYFS)
Development Matters in the Early Years Foundation Stage
Google Scholar
Evidence based pathways to intervention for children with language disorders
Profiles of strengths and weaknesses in dyslexia and other learning difficulties
Collaboration between teachers and speech and language therapists: services for primary school children with speech
Developing and testing EVALOE: a tool for assessing spoken language teaching and learning in the classroom
The influence of bilingualism on speech production: a systematic review
Teacher and speech language therapists collaboration: being equal and achieving a common goal
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Identification of fluency and word-finding difficulty in samples of children with diverse language backgrounds
Identification of biological and environmental risk factors for language delay: the Let’s Talk STEPS study
Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature
Literacy outcomes for students with speech impairment: long-term follow-up
The behaviour and self-esteem of children with specific speech and language difficulties
Self-esteem of adolescents with specific language impairment as they move from compulsory education
Meeting the needs of children and young people with speech
An evaluation of the use of accredited baseline assessment schemes in England
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
The consistency of baseline assessment schemes as measures of early literacy
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Speech and language therapy services to education in England and Wales
An evaluation of the written texts of children with SLI: the contributions of oral language
reading and phonological short-term memory
‘It’s the way you talk to them.’ The child’s environment: early years practitioners’ perceptions of its influence on speech and language development
its assessment and environment targeted interventions
‘I wasn’t trained to work with them’: mainstream teachers’ attitudes to children with speech and language difficulties
Advancing research on children with speech-language impairment: an introduction to the early childhood longitudinal study-Kindergarten Cohort
Barriers to collaboration: an analysis of systemic barriers to collaboration between teachers and speech and language therapists
Factors related to sustained implementation of schoolwide positive behavior support
Google Scholar
Prevalence of communication disorders compared with other learning needs in 14 500 primary and secondary school students
and training needs of early years professionals in relation to children’s speech and language development
Hidden language impairments in children: parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment
National Childcare Accreditation Council
Google Scholar
Younger children experience lower levels of language competence and academic progress in the first year of school: evidence from a population study
Evaluation of three proposed markers for language impairment in English: a meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy studies
Prologue—multiple perspectives for determining the roles of speech-language pathologists in inclusionary classrooms
Inequalities in the provision of paediatric speech and language therapy services across London boroughs
The working practices and clinical experiences of paediatric speech and language therapists: a national UK survey
Public Health England. (2016). The Mental Health of Children and Young People in England. Available at: https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/575632/Mental_health_of_children_in_England.pdf
Google Scholar
R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing
Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing
Google Scholar
Predicting stuttering onset by the age of 3 years: a prospective
Specific language impairment: a convenient label for whom
Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: findings from early language in Victoria Study
The Stuttering Severity Instrument for Adults and Children (SSI-4)
Google Scholar
Using lexical profiling tools to investigate children’s written vocabulary in grade 3: an exploratory study
attitudes and beliefs of the mainstream teachers of children with a preschool diagnosis of speech/language impairment
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sandwell Primary Care Trust
Wellcomm: A Speech and Language Toolkit for the Early Years
Google Scholar
Establishing language benchmarks for children with typically developing language and children with language impairment
Can early years professionals determine which preschoolers have comprehension delays
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Speech-language pathologists’ assessment practices for children with suspected speech sound disorders: results of a national survey
Input to interaction to instruction: three key shifts in the history of child language research
The dyslexia spectrum—continuities between reading
Better Communication Research Project: Language and Literacy Attainment of Pupils during Early Years and through KS2: Does Teacher Assessment at Five Provide a Valid Measure of Children’s Current and Future Educational Attainments
Google Scholar
emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI)
Evidence of ethnic disproportionality in special education in an english population
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Therneau, T., Atkinson, B., and Ripley, B. (2015). rpart: Recursive Partitioning and Regression Trees. Available at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/rpart/index.html
Google Scholar
Recurrent involuntary imagery in people who stutter and people who do not stutter
The influence of socio-economic status on mothers’ volubility and responsiveness in a monolingual Dutch-speaking sample
Screening for speech and language delay in children 5 years old and younger: a systematic review
Understanding the active ingredients in an effective preschool vocabulary intervention: an exploratory study of teacher and child talk during book reading
A comparison of inter-professional education programs in preparing prospective teachers and speech and language pathologists for collaborative language-literacy instruction
Paucity and disparity in kindergarten oral vocabulary instruction
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Leung D and Fugard AJB (2017) Children with Speech Language and Communication Needs in England: Challenges for Practice
Received: 27 February 2017; Accepted: 26 June 2017; Published: 19 July 2017
Copyright: © 2017 Dockrell, Howell, Leung and Fugard. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY)
provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited
*Correspondence: Julie Elizabeth Dockrell, ai5kb2NrcmVsbEB1Y2wuYWMudWs=
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or goodLearn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Open image viewerThe crash prompted a shutdown of national road 9 to all traffic on Tuesday
Image: Isto Janhunen / YleYle News23.2.2022 10:27The three victims of a deadly collision between a private car and truck in Jyväskylä on Tuesday morning were students at Humak University of Applied Sciences
The three young adults were driving on national road 9 from Korpilahti towards Jyväskylä when their car collided with a lorry at just after 8am
Three of the people in the car died in the crash
Määttä said that the school was offering crisis assistance to students and staff members
The institution also planned to hold an assembly for students surrounding the tragedy on Wednesday morning
this is a very tragic blow," Määttä said
expressing his deep condolences to the victims' relatives
All six of Humak's campuses observed a day of mourning on Wednesday
Police have not yet confirmed the identities of the victims
An accident investigation is still in its infancy
there were warnings in place for potentially dangerous driving conditions across southern and central Finland on Tuesday morning
Herman and Elisabeth Hallonblad – Art Patrons from Ladoga Karelia
The state councilman Herman Hallonblad (1825–1894) and his wife Elisabeth Hallonblad (1831–1907) from Sortavala were the most significant supporters of the Finnish and Karelian culture
Ladoga Karelia was formerly an area of Finland which was ceded to the Soviet Union after the Second World War
Herman Hallonblad was the mayor of Käkisalmi in Karelia and worked as a bank manager in Helsinki
He was the owner of three estates in Karelia and one in Häme
In 1851 he married Lisinka (Elisabeth) Siitoin
The Hallonblad family financed the lumber and metal plants
They also valued education and were establishing the Sortavala teacher seminar in Karelia
Last year marked the 100th anniversary of the Hallonblad donation to Finnish Art Association
The donation consists of 13 pieces of art from Hallonblad’s home
sculptures and prints were bought by the Finnish Art Association with Hallonblad’s funds
The entire collection consists of 185 works of art
which are now part of the Ateneum Art Museum’s collection
In Jyväskylä exhibition there are shown 42 paintings and sculptures of the most significant Finnish artists from 1860s –1930s
The collection includes treasures such as Helene Schjerfbeck’sT he Primary School Girl II (1908) and Self-Portrait
Ferdinand von Wright’s Capercaillies courting (1862) and The Surprise (1880)
Otto Mäkilä’s Summer Night (1938) as well as Tyko Sallinen’s The "Ducat" Girl (1914) and The Nude (1911)
An interesting section of this exhibition is dedicated to portraits and the artists’ self-portraits
Central Finland is also a common denominator for several of the pieces in this collection
Akseli Gallen-Kallela painted the first version of First Lesson
in the spirit of Runeberg in Keuruu in 1887
even though the painting was later finished in Paris in 1889
Yrjö Ollila’s Shepherdess (1915) was painted in Korpilahti
the exhibition includes Korpilahti born Alvar Cawén’s painting Mimosa (1914)
The acquisition of the exhibition was initiated by the Sortavala Foundation
This touring exhibition was brought together by the Ateneum Art Museum
the Joensuu Art Museum and the Jyväskylä Art Museum
The research related to this exhibition was done by M.Phil
The Collection was exhibited in Joensuu on 19 November 2009 – 16 May 2010
The Ateneum Art Museum will show this collection on 10 October 2010 – 14 March 2011
Gallery RatamoVeturintallinkatu 640100 JyväskyläOpening hours: Tue-Sun 11-18+358 (0)50 311 8901[email protected]
Art Museum OfficeVapaudenkatu 28PL 165 40101 Jyväskylä[email protected]
FacebookInstagramYouTubeVimeo
Jyväskylä museumsVisit Jyväskylä RegionJyväskylän kaupunki