and was written by student Samantha Chiang
Albany Middle School teacher James Izumizaki died recently
Albany High School students joined the rest of the community in a frenzied online discussion on September 26
when news emerged that middle school teacher and coach James Izumizaki had been arrested
who had just begun his fifth year of teaching at Albany Middle
faced accusations of committing lewd acts with a student
The news was impossible for students to ignore
appearing everywhere from the local news to social media feeds
Hundreds of comments were posted in response to online articles
and blocks of text replaced scattered thoughts in personal statuses as each student weighed in with his or her own opinion on the events that had occurred
if not unthinkable that any other event could rival the reaction generated by the arrest of the well-known Albany teacher
Albany students and their families were informed by a startling robo-call that Izumizaki had passed away as a result of what was reported to be a suspected suicide
the community took to social media to express a broad range of opinions
The role of media in the aftermath of these events created an unprecedented response
found a platform to exercise their freedom of speech through Facebook
The diversity of responses sparked further discussion about the overwhelming influence of the media in the tiny town
The community began to raise questions as to whether this freedom was a positive
or simply inevitable aspect of a changing society
Anyone who wanted a say in the issue could easily be heard
“The media is just another voice that people can use to express themselves,” commented Albany High Assistant Principal Susan Charlip
the media also provided opportunity for people to “get swept up without thinking about the larger picture.”
The many unanswerable questions in Izumizaki’s story created voids that were quickly filled by an onslaught of responses from students
and other members of the community who found an outlet to place blame on the media
and the school district were among the many groups that found themselves being blamed online for what had happened in thr Izumizaki case
Some of the answers created by the online Albany community escalated into full-fledged battles between those who viewed the situation in opposite ways
Some showed indignation on behalf of Izumizaki
and expressed anger towards those who even suggested that Izumizaki was guilty
Others clearly supported the young accusers
criticising Izumizaki’s supporters for being disrespectful to the supposed victims
Charlip described the discussion surrounding the events as “a natural way [for students] to make sense of their lives.”
The virtual reaction to Izumizaki’s arrest and death demonstrated the overpowering influence of the media that has only recently existed become prominent in society
Events and media have become increasingly dependent on each other
as those who chose to participate in the discussion each contribute to the original story
“We live in an age where we make the news ourselves
Charlip expressed belief that the media itself bears no responsibility for any individual’s actions
and is simply a modern-day “den” for discussion
The freedom and availability of the media entrusts a level of responsibility in the hands of its users
Surowitz explained that a tangled situation like the one that Albany faced requires students
“The media is just a piece of a really complicated story,” Surowitz concluded
Get more local news delivered straight to your inbox. Sign up for free Patch newsletters and alerts.
Metrics details
Electronic devices have become an indispensable part of our daily lives
while their negative aspects have been reported
One disadvantage is that reading comprehension is reduced when reading from an electronic device; the cause of this deficit in performance is unclear
we investigated the cause for comprehension decline when reading on a smartphone by simultaneously measuring respiration and brain activity during reading in 34 healthy individuals
reading on a smartphone elicits fewer sighs
promotes brain overactivity in the prefrontal cortex
reading on a smartphone affected sigh frequency but not normal breathing
suggesting that normal breathing and sigh generation are mediated by pathways differentially influenced by the visual environment
A path analysis suggests that the interactive relationship between sigh inhibition and overactivity in the prefrontal cortex causes comprehension decline
These findings provide new insight into the respiration-mediated mechanisms of cognitive function
visual input might also affect brain state and physiological condition
brain activity and physiological changes likely exist as mediating variables in the relationship between the visual environment and cognitive performance
These findings indicate that vision is dominant over other senses
and it is quite possible that visual environments influence various functions including that of the respiratory system
it is unclear how the visual environment affects respiration and brain function
this study investigated the involved mechanism in an exploratory manner
we aimed to explore and demonstrate the relationship between the visual environment and respiration and that between the visual environment and brain function to elucidate the phenomenon postulating that the use of digital devices lowers cognitive performance
If a particular visual environment negatively affects respiratory function and/or brain function, coupled with the interactive relationship between brain and respiratory activity, it is possible that the said process negatively influences cognitive performance (Supplementary Fig. 1)
This exploratory study examined the effects of electronic device use on reading comprehension by measuring brain and respiratory activity simultaneously in 34 healthy individuals
Reading score is decreased by using smartphone compared to paper medium
(a) Each trial consisted of four sessions: resting state before reading
and two min were spent in the resting state before/after reading
while the main effects in novel and the interaction were not
Scores with the smartphone medium were lower than those with the paper medium in sentences from both novel A and B (*P < 0.05)
d) No main or interaction effect was observed for medium or novel in either the duration of reading session or the viewing distance between participants’ eyes and the device
Error bars show standard error of the mean
Sighs are inhibited during reading with smartphone compared to paper
(a) A typical example of a raw respiration signal from a single participant
(b) A representative pattern of the tidal volume and sighs before
and after the reading sessions using a smartphone and paper media from a single participant
and expiratory time reduced during the reading sessions compared to those before and after sessions
(f) The respiratory frequency increased during the reading session compared to that before and after sessions (*P < 0.05)
(g) The number of sighs increased during the reading session with the paper medium compared to when using a smartphone and increased in the after reading session with both media (*P < 0.05)
Brain activity around the forehead is increased during reading with a smartphone
(a) A representative pattern of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) activity on the left probe before
c) RM-ANOVA showed that activity recorded with NIRS increased during the reading sessions with a smartphone medium from the left and right probes compared to that before and after reading sessions
The difference between the media was clearly visible in the left probe (*P < 0.05)
Six respiratory indexes (minute ventilation
two metabolic patterns (O2 consumption and End tidal CO2)
two NIRS indexes (right/left) in the reading sessions
and reading score were set as the observed variables
In the most suitable model (the goodness of fit index = 0.881)
there is a covariate relationship between left NIRS channel activity and the number of sighs (P = 0.021)
and a direct relationship between the right NIRS channel activity and its impact on reading score (P = 0.003)
Numbers mean standardized path coefficients
the number of sighs increased during cognitive reading activity on a paper medium and decreased when reading on a smartphone
This finding suggests that reading on a smartphone may have caused inhibition of sighs compared to reading on a paper medium
a path analysis suggests that the interactive relationship between sigh inhibition and overactivity in the prefrontal cortex causes the comprehension decline
Sighing may be associated with improved executive functions
These results suggest that a decline in reading comprehension on a smartphone may be caused
by reduced sighing and increased prefrontal activity compared to that on a paper medium
and activation of the preBötC receptor increases sighing by approximately 10 times
whereas inhibition of the receptor abolishes sighing
manipulation of the receptors had no effect on normal breathing
confirming that normal breathing and sighing are mediated by different pathways
our findings suggest that visual environment of reading affects sigh generation but not normal breathing
The decreased sighing and brain overactivity in smartphone use may be caused by sustained cognitive load in ipRGC activity due to blue light exposure
Further research is needed to examine the effect of blue light on respiration and brain activity
our experiment did not entail any measurement of subjective cognitive load
Based on the differences in the number of sighs and brain activity between reading on smartphones and paper media
it is highly likely that there might have been a difference in cognitive load as well
it is necessary to assess cognitive load indices and examine the relationship between breathing and brain activity
we did not control the movements when turning pages or pointing movements to maintain the focus of attention on the text
These bodily movements may have had some influence on the present index
such physical limitations should be taken into consideration
This study was approved by the ethics committee of Showa University School of Medicine and conducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (trial identifier number: 2179)
Thirty-four Japanese university students provided written informed consent prior to the experiments
All participants were right-hand dominant and had no history of neurological or psychiatric disease (20 females; mean age = 20.4
Participants had normal vision with/without correction
based on the condition that medium type and novel type did not overlap
A trial consisted of four sessions: resting state before reading
Participants were instructed to sit and their torso and arms were secured
They were asked to read the allocated novel on the allocated medium during the reading session
participants were asked to open their eyes and look at a wall
they took a reading test consisting of ten questions related to the contents of the novels
They were also asked to breathe through their nose during all sessions
and two min was spent in the resting state before/after reading
participants were asked whether they had previously read the novels used in the experiment
All participants responded that they had never read the novels
The observation distance was determined by the convenience of the participants
and the experimenter measured the distance between the device and the participants’ eyes
The factors of medium and novel had no influence on reading time or viewing distance
Each novel originated from a passage of one of two novels written by the same author
3060 Japanese characters; or novel B: Colorless Tsukuru Tazaki and His Years of Pilgrimage
The panel size of the smartphone was 5.0 inches (resolution pixel: height: 1920
The text size on the paper medium was identical to that on the smartphone
the weight (148 g) and outer frame (height: 144 mm
thickness: 8.6 mm) of the paper medium were also identical to those of the smartphone
The NIRS calculated oxygenated hemoglobin concentration [mM] on the forehead
The probe consisted of an LED that emits light and a photodiode that receives the light transmitted through the body
The amount of oxidized and deoxidized hemoglobin was estimated by quantifying the spatial slope of light scattering and transmission in body tissues
The interval duration between sighs was analyzed and the analysis was limited to cases in which two or more sighs during reading occurred within a single trial
RM-ANOVA and post-hoc t-tests were performed to test the main effects and interactions of medium (smartphone and paper) and novel sentence (novels A and B) on the scores for the reading test
They were also performed for medium (smartphone and paper) and session conditions (before
and after reading) on respiration (tidal volume
and number of sighs) and brain function (right and left activities of the prefrontal cortex)
Results are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean
The statistical significance criterion was defined as adjusted P < 0.05 with Bonferroni correction
Relationships among the 6 respiration indexes
and the comprehension score were calculated by path analysis
The goodness of fit of models was determined with the root-mean square of approximation
Supplementary information is provided with the paper to support the current results
Analyses used in this study are largely standard approaches for this type of data
Other data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request
Jaiswal, S. et al. Ocular and visual discomfort associated with smartphones, tablets and computers: What we do and do not know. Clin. Exp. Optom. 102, 463–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/cxo.12851 (2019)
Golebiowski, B. et al. Smartphone use and effects on tear film, blinking and binocular vision. Curr. Eye Res. 45, 428–434. https://doi.org/10.1080/02713683.2019.1663542 (2020)
Effects of VDT and paper presentation on consumption and production of information: Psychological and physiological factors
Schneps, M. H., Brockmole, J. R., Sonnert, G. & Pomplun, M. History of reading struggles linked to enhanced learning in low spatial frequency scenes. PLoS ONE 7, e35724. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0035724 (2012)
Skeel, R. L., Schutte, C., van Voorst, W. & Nagra, A. The relationship between visual contrast sensitivity and neuropsychological performance in a healthy elderly sample. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 696–705. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390590954173 (2006)
Spence, C. & Frings, C. Multisensory feature integration in (and out) of the focus of spatial attention. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 82, 363–376. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01813-5 (2020)
Driver, J. & Spence, C. Cross-modal links in spatial attention. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 353, 1319–1331. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1998.0286 (1998)
Honma, M. et al. Impairment of cross-modality of vision and olfaction in Parkinson disease. Neurology 90, e977–e984. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000005110 (2018)
Steele, J. D., Currie, J., Lawrie, S. M. & Reid, I. Prefrontal cortical functional abnormality in major depressive disorder: A stereotactic meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 101, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2006.11.009 (2007)
Structure-function correlates of cognitive decline in aging
Mizuno, K. et al. Less efficient and costly processes of frontal cortex in childhood chronic fatigue syndrome. Neuroimage Clin. 9, 355–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2015.09.001 (2015)
Vlemincx, E., Van Diest, I. & Van den Bergh, O. A sigh following sustained attention and mental stress: Effects on respiratory variability. Physiol. Behav. 107, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.05.013 (2012)
Grassmann, M., Vlemincx, E., von Leupoldt, A., Mittelstadt, J. M. & Van den Bergh, O. Respiratory changes in response to cognitive load: A systematic review. Neural Plast. 2016, 8146809. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/8146809 (2016)
Jerath, R. & Beveridge, C. Respiratory rhythm, autonomic modulation, and the spectrum of emotions: The future of emotion recognition and modulation. Front. Psychol. 11, 1980. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01980 (2020)
Heck, D. H., Kozma, R. & Kay, L. M. The rhythm of memory: How breathing shapes memory function. J. Neurophysiol. 122, 563–571. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00200.2019 (2019)
Zelano, C. et al. Nasal respiration entrains human limbic oscillations and modulates cognitive function. J. Neurosci. 36, 12448–12467. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2586-16.2016 (2016)
Biskamp, J., Bartos, M. & Sauer, J. F. Organization of prefrontal network activity by respiration-related oscillations. Sci. Rep. 7, 45508. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep45508 (2017)
Fujino, M., Ueda, Y., Mizuhara, H., Saiki, J. & Nomura, M. Open monitoring meditation reduces the involvement of brain regions related to memory function. Sci. Rep. 8, 9968. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-28274-4 (2018)
Maric, V., Ramanathan, D. & Mishra, J. Respiratory regulation & interactions with neuro-cognitive circuitry. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 95–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.001 (2020)
Jaiswal, D., Chowdhury, A., Banerjee, T. & Chatterjee, D. Effect of mental workload on breathing pattern and heart rate for a working memory task: A pilot study. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2202–2206, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2019.8856458 (2019)
Wang, K. et al. Left posterior prefrontal regions support domain-general executive processes needed for both reading and math. J. Neuropsychol. 14, 467–495. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12201 (2020)
Patael, S. Z. et al. Brain basis of cognitive resilience: Prefrontal cortex predicts better reading comprehension in relation to decoding. PLoS ONE 13, e0198791. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0198791 (2018)
Mandrick, K., Peysakhovich, V., Rémy, F., Lepron, E. & Causse, M. Neural and psychophysiological correlates of human performance under stress and high mental workload. Biol. Psychol. 121, 62–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.002 (2016)
Melnychuk, M. C. et al. Coupling of respiration and attention via the locus coeruleus: Effects of meditation and pranayama. Psychophysiology 55, e13091. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13091 (2018)
Doll, A. et al. Mindful attention to breath regulates emotions via increased amygdala-prefrontal cortex connectivity. Neuroimage 134, 305–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.041 (2016)
Vlemincx, E. et al. Respiratory variability and sighing: A psychophysiological reset model. Biol. Psychol. 93, 24–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.12.001 (2013)
Vlemincx, E. et al. Why do you sigh? Sigh rate during induced stress and relief. Psychophysiology 46, 1005–1013. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00842.x (2009)
Looking for inspiration: New perspectives on respiratory rhythm
A unitary analysis of pulmonary volume receptors
Characteristics and rate of occurrence of spontaneous and provoked augmented breaths
Li, P. et al. The peptidergic control circuit for sighing. Nature 530, 293–297. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16964 (2016)
Fernandez, D. C. et al. Light affects mood and learning through distinct retina-brain pathways. Cell 175, 71–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.08.004 (2018)
Short-wavelength sensitivity for the direct effects of light on alertness
and the waking electroencephalogram in humans
Janků, K., Šmotek, M., Fárková, E. & Kopřivová, J. Block the light and sleep well: Evening blue light filtration as a part of cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia. Chronobiol. Int. 37, 248–259. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2019.1692859 (2020)
Vlemincx, E., Taelman, J., Van Diest, I. & Van den Bergh, O. Take a deep breath: the relief effect of spontaneous and instructed sighs. Physiol. Behav. 101, 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.04.015 (2010)
Vlemincx, E., Van Diest, I. & Van den Bergh, O. A sigh of relief or a sigh to relieve: The psychological and physiological relief effect of deep breaths. Physiol. Behav. 165, 127–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.07.004 (2016)
Massar, S. A. A. et al. Reopening after lockdown: The influence of working-from-home and digital device use on sleep, physical activity, and wellbeing following COVID-19 lockdown and reopening. Sleep https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/zsab250 (2021)
Gruba, G. et al. PaLS Study of sleep deprivation and mental health consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic among university students: A cross-sectional survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 9581. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18189581 (2021)
Kanayama, N. & Niwayama, M. Examiner’s finger-mounted fetal tissue oximetry. J. Biomed. Opt. 19, 067008. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.19.6.067008 (2014)
Soltanlou, M., Sitnikova, M. A., Nuerk, H. C. & Dresler, T. Applications of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) in studying cognitive development: The case of mathematics and language. Front. Psychol. 9, 277. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00277 (2018)
Masaoka, Y., Koiwa, N. & Homma, I. Inspiratory phase-locked alpha oscillation in human olfaction: Source generators estimated by a dipole tracing method. J. Physiol. 566, 979–997. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2005.086124 (2005)
Vlemincx, E., Meulders, M. & Luminet, O. A sigh of relief or a sigh of expected relief: Sigh rate in response to dyspnea relief. Psychophysiology 55, e12979. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12979 (2018)
Download references
This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP18K03185
conceptualized the experiment and wrote the original manuscript
All the authors participated in editing the manuscript
The authors declare no competing interests
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05605-0
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
a shareable link is not currently available for this article
The Journal of Physiological Sciences (2022)
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science
Seeking a familiar flavor of my time years ago in Tokyo
I visited Chinmaya Izumizaki Icchoume-ten in Naha for spicy mapo tofu over rice
The restaurant is the only Okinawa location of the 23-branch Chinmaya chain
I was a regular at Chinmaya’s original location in Gotanda District
Chinmaya Gotanda Higashiguchi Ekimae-ten unfortunately closed but I still have fond memories of this spot and their signature mapo tofu over rice
I can still taste the burn the dish left on my taste buds
The first branch was popular and mapo tofu over rice is considered the “taste of Gotanda,” so two other branches remain in the area
Missing the days when this spicy food helped me rejuvenate after a long day at the office
The Chinmaya location here is called Izumizaki Icchoume-ten
and you’ll find it near the south end of Kokusai Street nestled among business and government office buildings in Naha
Izumizaki Ichhoumen-ten is a small restaurant with a striking resemblance to the one I frequented in Gotanda
Over the entrance I could see the letters “陳麻家” (Chinmaya)
there were also letters “担々麺” (dandan noodles) along with the letters “陳麻飯” for mapo tofu over rice
The unexpected additional menu item made me a little anxious for what was in store
There was no counter with seats; seats were available at tables
I took one of the seats for single diners and ordered chinmahan (650 yen
I still remembered that the joint in Gotanda served up orders at a surprising pace
It probably took less than a minute to get an order out
but I was happy to see my chinmahan arriving within a few minutes
The chinmahan looked exactly like the one I remember from Gotanda
it even left the same spice and fire in my mouth I knew and loved
the spiciness almost felt like it might go over my tolerance
The flavor and fire of the dish lured me to continue and in five minutes my plate was cleared
Chinmaya Izumizaki Icchoume-ten’s spicy mapo tofu was a much-needed refresh and walk down memory lane for me
Though I’m sad that the restaurant I frequented in Tokyo is now gone
I’m relieved to know that I can head to the Naha location for a familiar taste and burn
you’ll definitely enjoy Chinmaya Izumizaki Icchoume-ten’s fiery offering
Read now >
Sign up for our weekly newsletter of articles from Japan, Korea, Guam, and Okinawa with travel tips, restaurant reviews, recipes, community and event news, and more.
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.556519
The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between olfactory recognition and morphological changes in olfactory brain regions including the amygdala
and medial frontal cortex in 27 elderly subjects and 27 younger healthy controls
The specific aim of the study was to determine which brain areas are associated with the initial decline of olfaction in elderly subjects
All subjects underwent magnetic resonance imaging to measure anatomical brain volume and cortical thickness
and subjects were assessed using tests of olfactory acuity and cognitive function measured with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment
Overall brain volume reductions were observed in elderly subjects compared with young healthy controls
but only reduction in the volume of the left hippocampus was associated with decreased olfactory ability
The parahippocampus of elderly subjects was not different from that of controls; the extent of the reduction of parahippocampus volume varied among individuals
and reduction in this region was associated with olfactory decline
parahippocampus thinning was associated with decreased olfactory function
The path analysis showed direct and indirect effects of hippocampus and parahippocampus volume on olfactory ability and that volume reductions in these areas were not associated with cognitive function
Parahippocampus volume reduction and thinning exhibited individual variation; this may be the first appearance of pathological changes and may lead to dysfunction in the connection of olfactory memory to the neocortex
Parahippocampus change may reflect the first sign of olfactory impairment prior to pathological changes in the hippocampus
It is possible that loss of olfactory recognition might be caused by a decrease in frontal cortex function
Exploratory analysis was performed for cortical thickness measurements of the para-HI
and we determined whether cortical thinning contributes to volume changes as well as olfactory ability
The study was approved by the Ethical Committees of Showa University School of Medicine
and all participants provided written informed consent prior to participation
Demographic data of young healthy controls and elderly subjects
Odor detection acuity and odor recognition acuity were evaluated in all subjects using the T&T olfaction test (Takasuna Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). The T&T test is well correlated with the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (Kondo et al., 1988). Details of the olfaction test have been described elsewhere (Masaoka et al., 2005)
the test is conducted with five odors (odor A
each 10 times more concentrated than the last; concentrations are labeled from −2 to + 5
The five odors are presented randomly but at the same concentration in each trial
The trials begin with the lowest concentration and are repeated with progressively higher concentrations
the subject is asked whether an odor was perceived
The concentration at which the odor is perceived but not identified is considered the “detection level.” As the concentration increases
The subject is required to identify each odor and describe the kind of odor
The concentration at which an odor is first identified is considered the “recognition level.” Each subject’s odor detection threshold is expressed as the average of all odor threshold scores (A + B + C + D + E/5)
The recognition threshold is expressed in the same manner
Higher scores indicate lower olfactory detection and recognition abilities
All subjects were instructed to complete the Japanese version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA-J) (Nasreddine et al., 2005) under the supervision of neurologists. The MoCA can detect MCI with greater sensitivity than the Japanese version of the Mini Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975)
the MoCA was adapted for measurement of cognitive function
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed at Ebara Hospital using a 3 Tesla MAGNETOM A Trio Tim Scanner (Siemens
Germany) with a 32-channel phased-array coil
The anatomical scan was acquired with a T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE sequence with the following parameters: 9 degree flip angle
field of view 256 mm and 176 slices with a voxel size of 1 mm3
T1-weighted images of all subjects were acquired with the same scanner at Ebara Hospital
olfactory detection and olfactory recognition levels were examined using the non-parametric Mann-Whitney test
Group comparisons for ICV and whole brain volume (WBV) were conducted using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with sex and years of education as covariates
Between-group differences in the left and right HI
and MFC volumes were assessed using ANCOVA
years of education and ICV according to FreeSurfer (FreeSurfer ICV) included as covariates
To determine whether a differential relationship existed between olfactory recognition ability and volume of ROIs between groups
Each model included the main effects of group and volume and group × volume interactions
with FreeSurfer ICV and years of education as covariates
The main effects of group × volume interactions were the outcomes of interest
the FreeSurfer ICV and the ICV measured with SPM (SPM ICV) were compared using a non-parametric Mann-Whitney U test
Regression analysis was performed to determine the correlation coefficient
Reanalysis using the SPM ICV was performed for all comparisons
Comparison of the SPM ICV between elderly subjects and young HCs was conducted by ANCOVA with sex and years of education as covariates
years of education and SPM ICV as covariates
General linear model was conducted with SPM ICV and years of education as covariates
A group comparison of cortical thickness was performed with ANCOVA with sex and years of education as covariates
The ICV was not included in the thickness analysis as a covariate
Elderly subjects had a lower ICV (F = 8.53
0.45) and lower volumes of the left HI (F = 15.01
There were no significant differences in the left para-HI (F = 0.02
0.06) between the elderly subjects and young HCs
Table 2 shows comparisons of the OFC, para-HI, rectus and MFC thickness between the two groups. No correlation was observed between thickness of an ROI and ICV (FreeSurfer and SPM) (Supplementary Table 1); therefore
ICV was not used as covariate for analysis of cortical thickness comparisons
Similar results to the volume comparison were observed for cortical thickness
There was no significant difference in the left para-HI (F = 1.23
Significant decreases in the left OFC (F = 17.5
0.09) were observed in elderly subjects compared with young HCs
Comparison of cortical thickness between young HC and elderly subjects
Figure 1. Relationships between volumes of the hippocampus (HI) and amygdala (AMG) and olfactory test scores in elderly and young subjects (elderly subjects, closed circles with solid line; young subjects, open circles with dotted line). A significant group × volume interaction was observed for the left HI. There were no group × volume interactions for any other brain regions (statistical details are included in Supplementary Table 2)
Relationships between volumes of the parahippocampus (para-HI) and rectus and olfactory test scores in elderly and young subjects
A significant group × volume interaction was observed for the left para-HI
Relationships between volumes of the olfactory cortex (OFC) and medial prefrontal cortex (MFC) and olfactory test scores in elderly and young subjects
A general linear model was used to examine the effect of thickness and the group × cortical thickness interaction on olfactory recognition in elderly subjects and young HCs. A significant group × volume interaction was found for the left para-HI (P = 0.001) and right para-HI (P = 0.02) (Supplementary Figure 1, statistical details in Supplementary Table 3)
This small bias need to concern when the ICV is used as a covariate for ANCOVA and analysis with linear models
Because the path analysis included the HI and AMG volume
we considered that the same unit (mm3) could be used to help to understand the comparison
We focused on volume changes in the following analysis
Path diagram and standardized path coefficients for elderly subjects
The solid lines indicate a significant direct effect
The dotted line indicates a significant indirect effect
Gray solid lines indicate a non-significant direct path
and the gray dotted line indicates a non-significant indirect path
Our specific interest was to determine which olfactory areas were related to the decline of olfactory ability in elderly subjects who self-reported no cognitive impairment
Our aim was to test whether morphological changes might be related to the changes in olfactory ability that occur before cognitive decline
The most interesting findings in this study were observed in path analysis
indicating that direct and indirect effects of HI and para-HI volume on olfactory ability
Volume reductions in these areas were not associated with cognitive function measured by the MoCA
This means that subjects with smaller HI and para-HI volumes presented with impaired olfactory abilities prior to the decline of cognitive function
Overall brain volume reductions were observed in elderly subjects compared with young HCs
but only reduction in the volume of the left HI was associated with decreased olfactory ability
it was interesting to observe that the volume of the para-HI in elderly subjects was not different from that of young HCs; on the other hand
the extent of the reduction of para-HI volume varied among individuals
and this reduction was associated with olfactory decline
these findings suggest that the association between left HI and left para-HI volume changes might play a key role in olfactory recognition
The para-HI may have important roles in relaying memory retrieval and emotional reaction to conscious awareness of the context organized in the OFC
It is possible that volume changes of the HI and para-HI caused by pathological changes might contribute to the decline of olfactory recognition
where they play important roles in the relay areas projecting to the OFC
Olfactory decline was associated with HI and para-HI volume changes in this study, especially in the left hemisphere. Left HI volume reduction associated with memory and cognitive function has been reported at the onset of AD (Peng et al., 2015) and psychiatric disorders (Velakoulis et al., 1999; Wannan et al., 2019)
It is unknown why the left medial temporal regions are damaged prior to the onset of typical disease symptoms
Further research in a longitudinal study could help to determine whether current findings are relevant to MCI and AD patients
In our study, similar results to the volume comparison were observed for cortical thickness. However, the right rectus thinning was observed in elderly subjects. Volume reduction of the right rectus did not reach significant although having trends (P = 0.06 and 0.07, FreeSurfer ICV and SPM ICV, respectively). Additionally, significant group × thickness interaction was found for right para-HI (general linear model, Supplementary Table 3) as well as the left para-HI
It could be possible to assume that cortical thinning might be more sensitive to detect brain change than the volume reduction
The right rectus and right para-HI were not survived for the results of the path analysis
a relation between cortical thinning and volume reduction was interesting theme for understanding brain pathology
Storsve et al. (2014) reported the importance of longitudinal studies for determining changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume with aging. They reported that volume changes were closely related to thickness changes and found that area and thickness changes appeared to have both overlapping and different effects on volume across the adult life span (Storsve et al., 2014)
The areas that first show cortical thinning or volume reductions with age are worth investigating
In addition, in our study smaller ICV was observed in elderly subjects. Regarding ICV reduction in elderly subjects, age-related volumetric decline progresses by the age of 75 years (Barnes et al., 2010). It is unknown how the amount that cortical thinning or volume reduction may primarily contribute to ICV volume changes. Storsve et al. (2014) reported a mixture of accelerating or decelerating brain changes with age
although brain volume reductions and cortical thinning were observed in most regions
there was no link to cognitive function and olfactory ability
para-HI volume reduction and thinning varied among individuals
and this small amount of individuality was reflected in olfactory impairment
It is not known how elderly subjects with olfactory decline progress to cognitive impairment
and whether the para-HI accelerate to reduce volume in the further stage
A longitudinal study is required to answer these questions
The relationships between olfactory ability and medial temporal sub-regions and hippocampal subfields also worthy of further investigation
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committees of Showa University School of Medicine
The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study
and MH conducted and analyzed olfactory testing and the MoCA
This study was supported by a JSPS KAKENHI Grant (Number 15K00210) and Kao Corporation
The authors declare that this study received funding from Kao Corporation
The funder had the following involvement with the study: HS was employed by Kao Corporation and was involved in collection and analysis of olfaction and MoCA data
We thank Katsutoshi Murata (Siemens Healthcare, Japan) for providing the MRI sequence prototype used in this study and also thank Terumi Tabei, Ken Yashiro, and Toshiyuki Onodera (Department of Radiology, Ebara Tokyo Hospital, Japan) for assistance with scanning. We also thank Melony Black, Ph.D. and Lisa Kreiner, Ph.D. from Edanz Group (https://en-author-services.edanzgroup.com/ac) for editing a draft of this manuscript
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2020.556519/full#supplementary-material
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
age and gender adjustment in MRI studies: a necessary nuisance
Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Frontal atrophy as a marker for dementia conversion in Parkinson’s disease with mild cognitive impairment
Google Scholar
Perfumers’ expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions
An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest
Olfactory dysfunction in parkinsonism: general deficit unrelated to neurologic signs
The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory
Google Scholar
Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images
Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain
Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex
sMini-mental state’: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician
Google Scholar
Left frontal cortex connectivity underlies cognitive reserve in prodromal Alzheimer disease
MR-based in vivo hippocampal volumetrics: 2
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Human adult neurogenesis: evidence and remaining questions
A study of the relationship between the T&T olfactometer and the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test in a Japanese population
Google Scholar
PET Tau and Amyloid-β Burden in Mild Alzheimer’s Disease: divergent relationship with age
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Accurate automatic estimation of total intracranial volume: a nuisance variable with less nuisance
Inspiratory phase-locked alpha oscillationin human olfaction: source generators estimated by a dipole tracing method
Markers of brain illness may be hidden in your olfactory ability: a Japanese Perspective
Impaired olfactory ability associated with larger left hippocampus and rectus volumes at earliest stages of schizophrenia: a sign of neuroinflammation
Olfactory impairment in Parkinson’s disease caused by weak activations of the limbic areas
Olfaction in neurodegenerative disease: a meta-analysis of olfactory functioning in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases
MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment
Declarative association in the perirhinal cortex
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Correlation of hippocampal volume and cognitive performances in patients with either mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s disease
Mitotically active cells that generate neurons and astrocytes are present in multiple regions of the adult mouse hippocampus
doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000828)424:3<397::aid-cne2<3.0.co;2-a
Association between olfactory dysfunction and amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease dementia
The rules of formation of the olfactory representations found in the orbitofrontal cortex olfactory areas in primates
Time course of odorant-induced activation in the human primary olfactory cortex
Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness
surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change
Google Scholar
Hippocampal volume in first-episode psychoses and chronic schizophrenia
Hippocampal subfields and visuospatial associative memory across stages of schizophrenia-spectrum disorder
Left posterior orbitofrontal cortex is associated with odor-induced autobiographical memory: an fMRI study
Ono K and Izumizaki M (2020) Hippocampus and Parahippocampus Volume Reduction Associated With Impaired Olfactory Abilities in Subjects Without Evidence of Cognitive Decline
Copyright © 2020 Kubota, Masaoka, Sugiyama, Yoshida, Yoshikawa, Koiwa, Honma, Kinno, Watanabe, Iizuka, Ida, Ono and Izumizaki. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY)
distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted
provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited
in accordance with accepted academic practice
distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms
*Correspondence: Yuri Masaoka, ZmF1c3R1c0BtZWQuc2hvd2EtdS5hYy5qcA==
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations
Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or goodLearn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish